Decision-making paradox

(Redirected from Decision making paradox)

The decision-making paradox is a phenomenon related to decision-making and the quest for determining reliable decision-making methods. It was first described by Triantaphyllou, and has been recognized in the related literature as a fundamental paradox in multi-criteria decision analysis (MCDA), multi-criteria decision making (MCDM) and decision analysis since then.

Description

edit

The decision-making paradox was first described in 1989,[1] and further elaborated in the 2000 book by Triantaphyllou on multi-criteria decision analysis (MCDA) / multi-criteria decision making (MCDM).[2] It arises from the observation that different decision-making methods, both normative and descriptive, yield different results, when fed with exactly the same decision problem and data.[citation needed] It has been recognized in the related literature as a fundamental paradox in multi-criteria decision analysis (MCDA) / multi-criteria decision making (MCDM), and decision analysis since then.[3][4][5][6][7][8][better source needed]

In a study reported in International Journal of Decision Support Systems[1] and Multi-Criteria Decision Making: A Comparative Study,[2] the following investigation was undertaken. Since in the beginning it was assumed that the best method is not known, the problem of selecting the best method was solved by successively using different methods. The methods used in that study were the weighted sum model (WSM), the weighted product model (WPM), and two variants of the analytic hierarchy process (AHP). It was found that when a method was used, say method X (which is one of the previous four methods), the conclusion was that another method was best (say, method Y). When method Y was used, then another method, say method Z, was suggested as being the best one, and so on.

Two evaluative criteria were used to formulate the previous decision-making problem, which is actually an MCDM problem. The first criterion was based on the premise that a method which claims to be accurate in multi-dimensional problems (for which different units of measurement are used to describe the alternatives), should also be accurate in single-dimensional problems. For such problems, the weighted sum model (WSM) is the widely accepted approach, thus, their results were compared with the ones derived from the WSM. The second evaluative criterion was based on the situation: alternative A, is evaluated as the best alternative, compared to the non-optimal alternative B. If B is replaced by a worse one, one should expect that alternative A remains the best alternative, under normal conditions where the weights of the two evaluative criteria in all possible combinations always add equal to 1. If not it is known as a ranking reversal.[2]

Methods affected

edit

The following multi-criteria decision-making methods have been confirmed to exhibit this paradox:The analytic hierarchy process (AHP) and some of its variants, the weighted product model (WPM), the ELECTRE (outranking) method and its variants and the TOPSIS method.[1][2]

Other methods

edit

Other methods that have not been tested yet but may exhibit the same phenomenon include the following:

A key role in this quest is played by the study of rank reversals in decision making.

References

edit
  1. ^ a b c Triantaphyllou, E.; S.H. Mann (1989). "An Examination of the Effectiveness of Multi-Dimensional Decision-Making Methods: A Decision-Making Paradox". International Journal of Decision Support Systems. 5 (3): 303–312. doi:10.1016/0167-9236(89)90037-7. Retrieved 2010-06-25.
  2. ^ a b c d Triantaphyllou, E. (2000). Multi-Criteria Decision Making: A Comparative Study. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers (now Springer). p. 320. ISBN 0-7923-6607-7.
  3. ^ Bernroider, E.W.N.; V. Stix (2006). "On The Applicability of Data Envelopment Analysis for Multiple Attribute Decision Making in the Context of Information Systems Appraisals". Data Envelopment Analysis for Multiple Attribute Decision Making, Communications of the IIMA 107. 6 (2): 107–118.
  4. ^ Caterino, N.; I. Iervolino; G. Manfredi; E. Cosenza (2009). "A Comparative Analysis of Multi-Criteria Decision-Making Methods for Seismic Structural Retrofitting". Computer-Aided Civil and Infrastructure Engineering. 24 (6): 1–14. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8667.2009.00599.x. S2CID 18689305.
  5. ^ Fitz-Gerald, A.; M. Tracy (2008). "Developing a Decision-Making Model for Security Sector Development in Uncertain Situations". Journal of Security Sector Management: 1–37.
  6. ^ Bernroider, E.W.N.; S J. Mitlöhner. "Social Choice Aggregation Methods for Multiple Attribute Business Information System Selection". Vienna University of Economics and Business Administration, Augasse 2–6, 1090 Vienna, Austria.
  7. ^ Mysiak, J. "Development of transferable multicriteria decision tools for water resource management". UFZ Centre for Environmental Research, Permoserstraße 15; 04318 Leipzig, Germany: 1–6.
  8. ^ Falessi, D.; Tutor: Prof. Giovanni Cantone; Coordinatore: Prof. Daniel P. Bovet. "A Toolbox for Software Architecture Design (a Doctoral Dissertation)". Universita Degli Studi di Roma Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy, Facoltà di Ingegneria, Dottorato di Ricerca in Informatica e Ingegneria, dell'Automazione, XX Ciclo: 1–203.