Numbami (also known as Siboma or Sipoma) is an Austronesian language spoken by about 200 people with ties to a single village in Morobe Province, Papua New Guinea. It is spoken in Siboma village (7°30′47″S 147°18′04″E / 7.512985°S 147.301128°E), Paiawa ward, Morobe Rural LLG.[2][3][4]
Numbami | |
---|---|
Native to | Papua New Guinea |
Region | coastal village in Morobe Province |
Native speakers | 200 (2007)[1] |
Language codes | |
ISO 639-3 | sij |
Glottolog | numb1247 |
ELP | Numbami |
Numbami is classified as Vulnerable by the UNESCO Atlas of the World's Languages in Danger | |
Numbami is a phonologically conservative isolate within the Huon Gulf languages, and is the last Austronesian language on the south coast of the Huon Gulf. Its nearest relatives along the coast to the southeast are 270 km away, Maisin and Arifama-Miniafia in Oro Province (Northern Province in the former colony of Papua).
The word order typology of Numbami and the Huon Gulf languages is subject–verb–object (SVO), which is typical of Austronesian languages; while that of Arifama-Miniafia and most of the Papuan Tip languages is subject–object–verb (SOV), which is typical of Papuan languages. Maisin has been characterized as a mixed language, with both Austronesian and Papuan features that obscure its primary heritage, and there is evidence that settlements of Austronesian speakers along the 270 km of coast were gradually absorbed into inland communities speaking Binanderean languages (Bradshaw 2017).
Phonology
editNumbami distinguishes 5 vowels and 18 consonants. Voiceless /s/ is a fricative, but its voiced and prenasalized equivalents are affricated, varying between more alveolar [(n)dz] and more palatalized [(n)dʒ]. The liquid /l/ is usually rendered as a flap [ɾ]. The labial approximant is slightly fricative, tending toward [β], when followed by front vowels.
Vowels (orthographic)
editFront | Central | Back | |
---|---|---|---|
High | i | u | |
Mid | e | o | |
Low | a |
Consonants (orthographic)
editBilabial | Dental | Alveopalatal | Velar | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Voiceless | p | t | s | k |
Voiced | b | d | z | g |
Prenasalized | -mb- | -nd- | -nz- | -ŋg- |
Nasal | m | n | ŋ | |
Liquid | l | |||
Approximant | w | y |
Obstruent harmony
editPrenasalized obstruents only occur in medial position, where the distinction between oral and prenasalized voiced obstruents is somewhat predictable. Medial voiced obstruents are statistically far more likely to be oral in words beginning with oral voiced obstruents, while they are far more likely to be prenasalized in words beginning with anything else. If denasalization of voiced obstruents is an ongoing change, one can track its progress through different lexical environments: it is 100% complete in word-initial position (as in bola 'pig' and buwa 'areca nut'), 80% complete in the middle of words beginning with voiced obstruents (as in bada 'market' and dabola 'head' vs. zanzami 'driftwood'), 35% complete in the middle of words beginning with approximants or vowels (as in ababa 'crosswise' vs. ambamba 'handdrum' and waŋga 'canoe'), not quite 20% complete in words beginning with voiceless obstruents (as in kaze 'left side' vs. kaimbombo 'butterfly' and pindipanda 'flea'), not quite 5% complete in words beginning with nasals (as in nomba 'thing' and -mande 'to die'), and not attested at all in words beginning with liquids (as in lenda 'nit' and lindami 'lime spatula'). (See Bradshaw 1978a.)
Morphology
editAlthough Numbami is phonologically conservative, it retains very little productive morphology, most of it related to person and number marking.
Pronouns and person markers
editFree pronouns
editFree pronouns occur in the same positions as subject or object nouns. They distinguish three persons (with a clusivity distinction in the first person) and four numbers (Bradshaw 1982a).
Person | Singular | Plural | Dual | Paucal |
---|---|---|---|---|
1st person inclusive | aita | atuwa | aito | |
1st person exclusive | woya | i | ilu(wa) | ito |
2nd person | aiya | amu | amula | amuto |
3rd person | e | ai | alu(wa) | aito |
Genitive pronouns
editGenitive pronouns also distinguish three persons (plus clusivity) and four numbers (Bradshaw 1982a).
Person | Singular | Plural | Dual | Paucal |
---|---|---|---|---|
1st person inclusive | aita-ndi | atuwa-ndi | aito-tandi | |
1st person exclusive | na-ŋgi | i-na-mi | ilu(wa)-mandi | ito-mandi |
2nd person | a-na-mi | amu-ndi | amula-mundi | amuto-mundi |
3rd person | e-na | ai-ndi | aluwa-ndi | aito-ndi |
Subject prefixes
editVerbs are marked with subject prefixes that distinguish three persons (plus clusivity) and two tenses, Nonfuture and Future. (The latter distinction is often characterized as one between Realis and Irrealis mode; see Bradshaw 1993, 1999.)
Person | Sing. Nonf. | Plur. Nonf. | Sing. Fut. | Plur. Fut. |
---|---|---|---|---|
1st person inclusive | ta- | tana- | ||
1st person exclusive | wa- | ma- | na- | mana- |
2nd person | u- | mu- | nu- | muna- |
3rd person | i- | ti- | ni- | ina- |
In most cases, subject prefixes are easily segmentable from verb stems, but in a few very high frequency cases, prefix-final vowels merge with verb-initial vowels to yield irregularly inflected forms, as in the following paradigm: wani (< wa-ani) '1SG-eat', woni (< u-ani) '2SG-eat', weni (< i-ani) '3SG-eat', tani (< ta-ani) '1PLINCL-eat', mani (< ma-ani) '1PLEXCL-eat', moni (< mu-ani) '2PL-eat', teni (< ti-ani) '3PL-eat'.
Numerals
editTraditional Numbami counting practices started with the digits of the left hand, then continued on the right hand and then the feet, to reach '20', which translates as 'one person'. Higher numbers are multiples of 'one person'. Nowadays, most counting above '5' is done in Tok Pisin. As in other Huon Gulf languages, the short form of the numeral 'one' functions as an indefinite article.
Numeral | Term | Gloss |
---|---|---|
1 | sesemi / te | 'one' |
2 | luwa | 'two' |
3 | toli | 'three' |
4 | wata | 'four' |
5 | nima teula | 'hands half/part' |
6 | nima teula ano sesemi | 'hands half right one' |
7 | nima teula ano luwa | 'hands half right two' |
8 | nima teula ano toli | 'hands half right three' |
9 | nima teula ano wata | 'hands half right four' |
10 | nima besuwa | 'hands both/pair' |
20 | tamota te | 'person one' |
Names
editLike many other Huon Gulf languages, Numbami has a system of birth-order names. The seventh son and sixth daughter are called "No Name": Ase Mou 'name none'.
Birth order | Sons | Daughters |
---|---|---|
1 | Alisa | Kale |
2 | Aliŋa | Aga |
3 | Gae | Aya |
4 | Alu | Damiya |
5 | Sele | Owiya |
6 | Dei | Ase Mou |
7 | Ase Mou |
Ideophones
editAlthough many languages have a class of ideophones with distinctive phonology, Numbami is unusual in having a morphological marker for such a class. The suffix -a(n)dala is unique to ideophones but is clearly related to the word andalowa 'path, way, road' (POc *jalan). (See Bradshaw 2006.) In the following examples, acute accents show the placement of word stress.
- bái-andala 'overcast, clouded over'
- dendende-ándala 'shivering'
- golópu-adala 'slipping or dripping through'
- kí-andala 'scorching, parched'
- paká-adala 'getting light, flashing on, popping'
- pilipíli-adala 'flapping, fluttering'
- sí-andala 'shooting up, springing away'
- sulúku-adala 'sucking, slurping'
- taká-adala 'stuck fast, planted firmly'
- tíki-adala 'going dark'
Syntax
editWord order
editThe basic word order in Numbami is SVO, with prepositions, preposed genitives, postposed adjectives and relative clauses. Relative clauses are marked at both ends, and so are some prepositional phrases. Negatives come at the ends of the clauses they negate. There is also a class of deverbal resultatives that follow the main verb (and its object, if any).
alu
3DU
ti-
3PL
lapa
hit
bola
pig
uni
dead
'The two of them killed a pig.'
naŋgi
GEN.1SG
gode
cousin
i-
3SG
loŋoni
hear
biŋa
talk
Numbami
Numbami
kote
not
'My cross-cousin doesn't understand Numbami.'
ma-
1PL.EXCL
ki
put
bani
food
manu
which
ma-
1PL.EXCL
yaki
pare
na
REL
su
into
ulaŋa
pot
'We (excl.) put the food we've pared into the pot.'
Possessive vs. attributive genitives
editTwo kinds of genitive modifiers precede their heads while one type follows its head noun (Bradshaw 1982a).
Whole-part genitives
editNoun-noun phrases denoting wholes and parts occur in the order stated, with the latter serving as head of the phrase: wuwu lau 'betel pepper leaf', tina daba 'headwater', nima daba (lit. 'hand head') 'thumb', kapala lalo (lit. 'house inside') 'indoors', Buzina bubusu 'Buzina (Salamaua) point'.
Possessive genitives
editGenitive possessor nouns precede their head nouns, with an intervening possessive marker that distinguishes singular (na) from plural (ndi) possessors: wuwu na lau 'the leaves of the (generic) betel pepper plant; particular betel pepper plant's leaf'; kapala na lalo 'the insides of (generic) houses; the inside of a particular house'; Siasi ndi gutu 'the Siassi Islands; islands belonging to a particular group of Siassi people'; bumewe ndi bani 'food typically eaten by whites; food belonging to a particular group of whites'.
Attributive genitives
editAttributive genitives resemble possessive genitives except that (1) the modifiers follow their heads, and (2) the "possessors" are nonreferential except in a generic sense, that is, they "never refer to a particular subset of the set they name" (Bradshaw 1982a:128): wuwu weni na 'forest (wild) betel pepper', wuwu Buzina ndi 'type of betel pepper associated with the Buzina people at Salamaua', walabeŋa tamtamoŋa na 'fish poison, native means of stunning fish', walabeŋa bumewe na 'explosives, European means of stunning fish'.
Verb serialization
editVerb serialization is very common in Numbami. Within a serial verb construction, all verbs must agree in tense. Subject choice in successive verbs is severely constrained. Noninitial subjects can only refer to preceding subjects, preceding objects, or preceding events or conditions, and only in that order (Bradshaw 1993). Negatives come at the ends of the clauses they negate.
tako,
enough
ma-
1PL.EXCL
woti
descend
ma-
1PL.EXCL
ma
come
ma-
1PL.EXCL
ŋgewe
carry
bani
food
'Okay, we come back down carrying food.'
aiya
2SG
nu-
FUT.2SG
kole
turn
nu-
FUT.2SG
ŋgo
say
biŋa
word
de
to
woya
1SG
kote
not
'Don't you turn around and talk to me.'
ina-
FUT.3PL
ki
put
damu
dry.frond
ni-
FUT.3SG
nzeka
lie.upon
kundu
sago.starch
'They'll put dry fronds on top of the sago starch.'
wa
and
aiya
2SG
nu-
FUT.2SG
ki
put
yawi
fire
ni-
FUT.3SG
soloŋa
enter
'And you'll set it afire.'
woya
1SG
wa-
1SG
yoŋgo
see
aiya
2SG
i-
3SG
muŋga
precede
'I saw you first.'
takalama
today
iluwa
1DU.EXCL
ma-
1PL.EXCL
yoŋgo
see
ata
self
i-
3SG
wete
count
-ma
ADV
kote
not
'Nowadays, we two don't see each other very regularly.'
External links
edit- Numbami (Siboma) Wordlist at the Austronesian Basic Vocabulary Database
- Numbami-English dictionary (PDF)
- English-Numbami dictionary (PDF)
- Recorded and written materials for Numbami are archived with Kaipuleohone
References
edit- ^ Numbami at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
- ^ Eberhard, David M.; Simons, Gary F.; Fennig, Charles D., eds. (2019). "Papua New Guinea languages". Ethnologue: Languages of the World (22nd ed.). Dallas: SIL International.
- ^ United Nations in Papua New Guinea (2018). "Papua New Guinea Village Coordinates Lookup". Humanitarian Data Exchange. 1.31.9.
- ^ English-Numbami dictionary
- Bradshaw, Joel (2017). Evidence of contact between Binanderean and Oceanic languages. Oceanic Linguistics 56:395–414.
- Bradshaw, Joel (2010). Manner and path verb serialization in Numbami. In A journey through Austronesian and Papuan linguistic and cultural space: Papers in honour of Andrew Pawley, ed. by John Bowden, Nikolaus P Himmelmann, and Malcolm Ross, pp. 441–453. Pacific Linguistics 615. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.
- Bradshaw, Joel (2006). Grammatically marked ideophones in Numbami and Jabêm. Oceanic Linguistics 45:53–63.
- Bradshaw, Joel (1999). Null subjects, switch-reference, and serialization in Jabêm and Numbami. Oceanic Linguistics 38:270–296.
- Bradshaw, Joel (1997). The population kaleidoscope: Another factor in the Melanesian diversity v. Polynesian homogeneity debate. Journal of the Polynesian Society 106:222–249.
- Bradshaw, Joel (1993). Subject relationships within serial verb constructions in Numbami and Jabêm. Oceanic Linguistics 32:133–161.
- Bradshaw, Joel (1982a). Genitives and relatives in Numbami, a New Guinea Austronesian language. In Gava‘: Studies in Austronesian languages and cultures dedicated to Hans Kähler, ed. by Rainer Carle, Martina Heinschke, Peter W. Pink, Christel Rost, and Karen Stadtlander, pp. 123–139. Veröffentlichungen des Seminars für Indonesische und Südseesprachen der Universität Hamburg, vol. 17. Berlin: Dietrich Reimer.
- Bradshaw, Joel (1982b). Word order change in Papua New Guinea Austronesian languages. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Hawaiʻi.
- Bradshaw, Joel (1978a). The development of an extra series of obstruents in Numbami. Oceanic Linguistics 17:39–76.
- Bradshaw, Joel (1978b). Multilingualism and language mixture among the Numbami. Kivung: Journal of the Linguistic Society of Papua New Guinea 11:26-49.
- Ross, Malcolm (1988). Proto Oceanic and the Austronesian languages of western Melanesia. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.