Olfactic communication

(Redirected from Olfactory communication)

Olfactic communication is a channel of nonverbal communication referring to the various ways people and animals communicate and engage in social interaction through their sense of smell. Our human olfactory sense is one of the most phylogenetically primitive[1] and emotionally intimate[2] of the five senses; the sensation of smell is thought to be the most matured and developed human sense.

Sharing the sense of smell
A tigress rubbing her head on a tree

Human ancestors essentially depended on their sense of smell to alert themselves of danger such as poisonous food and to locate potent mating partners. Using the sense of smell as an instrument paved a way for smell to become a platform of nonverbal communication. Smell also has a significant influence on social interactions. Through their branch of olfaction research, the National Science Foundation recorded that over 70 percent of American adults believe a person's body odor has a significant effect on how interested they will be when conversing with people of a different sex.[3] This process is possible with olfactory bulbs, the part of the brain that discriminates and enhances certain odors. Typically, women will prefer men whose natural odor is similar to their own, while heterosexual men are attracted to females with high estrogen levels and strong menstrual secretions.[4] An entire industry has been developed to provide people with personal smell-masking products, such as perfume, cologne, deodorant, and scented lotions. When a person covers their natural body odor with a pleasant smell, they are communicating their desire to be attractive either emotionally, sexually, or romantically.[3]

History

edit

Olfactics is the most critical sense of human biology for centuries, the first classification system for odors was not developed until the later half of the 1700s by Carolus Linnaeus; today, humans are most dependent on eyesight. Linnaeus' system was composed of seven different categories that various types of smells could be identified with. These categories are champhoraceous musky, floral, pepperminty, ethereal, pungent, and putrid.[5] This classification system has been readvised numerous times and is still being developed to actively interpret human olfactics. Linnaeus' work sparked interest in several colleagues and other scientists, leading to theories of how olfactics are linked to a person's mood and emotional state. This led to the creation of scented room vaporizer in 1851. Eugene Remmel's room perfumer was originally designed to uplift or relax the occupants of the space. However, this invention instead rose to popularity for its simple ability of providing suitable air quality by ventilating small, heavily populated public places. Today, people use scented essential oils in their home or office to create a certain ambience. For example, peppermint is used to uplift dreary attitudes, citrus attracts motivational energy, vanilla is used to promote calmness, and lavender predicts relaxation. Sharing a scent with the entire room spreads a message of what atmosphere the host would like to set and in return, nonverbally communicates the manner in which the guests or visitors should act.

 
Illustration of Henning's Smell PrismImage originally released by the Water Environment Federation in 1978

In 1916, Hans Henning created a three dimensional smell-prism with six corners. Each corner represented a distinctive smell including flowery or ethereal, putrid, fruity or fragrant, spicy, burned, and resinous. Henning's theory suggested all other smells were a combination of these six and each smell occupied its own location on the prism.[5] This prism led to new developments in understanding olfactics, however not everyone was satisfied with Henning's research. Today, there is still not a universally agreed upon standardized classification system for olfaction. This is primarily due to the drastic differences in how certain smells are perceived by various cultures. Although sufficient research is still being recorded to explain the connection between olfaction and preferences, experts have theorized that certain smells are connected to a person's thinking, creativity, memory, and reaction abilities. This is because when a person is experiencing a negative emotion, their olfaction sense sharpens. This creates a lasting memory, correlating the scent with a certain experience and emotions a person felt.[3] The same patterns can be detected when a person feels joy, sadness, or fear.

Person perception

edit

The attributions we make through the olfactic channel have implications for our moral understandings of ourselves and others, along with the judgments and evaluations we make about identity, social class, status, sex, race, gender and ethnic relationships.[6]

How smells impact interactions

edit

Olfactics or smell have incredibly varying interpretations depending on where a person is in the world. In some cultures negative smells can result in positive emotions where in the western world, negative smells are often met with negative emotions. The sense of smell has often been overlooked and understudied and many people often perceive olfactics as a lower importance sense as it pertains to communication. The importance and variation of olfactics can be seen in how different countries use the senses and how different cultures interpret different smells. For example, rot is often a very negative smell and is often met with a tightening reaction in the body including a restricting of the pupils in an attempt for the body to receive a reduced amount of information. However, as the smell may be met with a reaction of disgust in one country, another country may react with happiness as it is a way for some cultures to gauge whether food has finished going through a certain process or not.

Olfactics also can impact how someone is received in a social setting in different countries or cultures. In different regions of the world, it is necessary to develop different coding linguistics in order to survive in that area. Smell is one sense that is often poorly coded in human communication in western culture[7] because humans have a hard time identifying smells and communicating them.[8] Researcher Asifa Majid at the University of York argues that there is cross cultural evidence that there are numerous languages that utilize coding olfaction in their language. These languages have encoded smell into their grammar due to their environmental needs.[9] The Umpila are Indigenous people of Australia that have the most codeable olfactory sense in their language.[7]

Due to the impact smell has on the human brain and the reception of different people, it can impact whether or not an individual accepts another individual. Bad smells can drive people away due to the harsh nature of the odors, whereas nice smelling fragrances can communicate positively.[10] During medieval times peasants would often give off horrid odors and the higher noble classes would often look down on them for it, and while the reaction may not be as harsh, the same instances can be seen in today's world. Big corporations can manipulate the olfactic senses in customers or employees by having a nice scent being put out in order to give off feelings of comfort or to promote attitudes of productivity.

Impacts other senses

edit

Scent can transmit an intrinsic message. It can also transmit a message that relies on, or affects, other senses, such as:

Touch

edit

The interactions between the touch and olfactic senses will dictate behavior due to the circumstances of where the interaction is set to occur. It could also be used with smell in an attempt to identify what a random or unknown object could be. For example, If a person were to walk into a room that did not smell good they would be less likely to interact with the environment around them. If a room smells welcoming and comforting a person would be more likely to stay longer and interact with more things. These examples provide a brief understanding on how smell may affect the behaviors or interactions behind certain senses.[11]

Taste

edit

Taste and smell go hand in hand as some of the main stimulants for flavor is the result that comes from smell working with taste. The human body has many complex interactions when it comes to how the five senses can help or hurt each other. These two senses can impact people's emotions and behaviors and can also cause pain depending on what the sense is being used for.[12] These senses can also be positively or negatively affected by medication, disease, smoking, or drinking. For example, COVID-19 was well known for temporarily nullifying the sense of taste and smell.[13] Olfactics can help dictate on whether or not something should be consumed, but at times the sense of smell can be fooled.

Sight

edit

In a large survey study on the importance and attention to olfaction in daily life, Wrzesniewski et al. found that completely losing one's sense of smell was ranked as equivalent to losing one's little left toe or hearing in one ear.[14] Sight is a common sense but when it comes to smelling what a person is surrounded by (i.e., environment). The sensory stimuli can affect a person's behavior when browsing through a grocery store, even in the environment. Odor can affect individuals subconscious mind and make a consumer become more allured to specific things.[15] Visual dominance refers not only to neural processing but also to vision's cultural and social primacy.

Sound

edit

With olfactics we perceive auditory cues in our everyday life.[16] For instance: a person who struggles with halitosis. While talking to others, since people experience not only odors through the front of the nose or the back of the nose, but also sounds, auditory perceptions may be neglected or drawn out by the sense of smell. Of the five senses sound has the least impact on olfactic communication. With sound a person may be able to hear the sound of food cooking, though they must use their olfactory epithelium which contains special receptors that are sensitive to odor molecules that travel through the air.[17]

COVID-19 pandemic

edit

As of March 2020, the COVID-19 outbreak left many individuals losing their sense of smell and/or taste, those that have had the feeling of loss in their senses were told to quarantine or severe isolation. Many people still to modern day are battling with loss of smell and taste and have upper respiratory problems. During the COVID-19 outbreak, patients with sudden loss of smell should initiate social distancing and home isolation measures and be tested for SARS-CoV-2 diagnostic test when available. Olfactory training is recommended when smell does not come back after one month but can be started earlier.[18] The leading cause to the loss of smell is URTI, Smell loss in URTI is caused by a multifactorial combination of mechanical obstruction for the odorant transmission in the olfactory cleft due to mucosal inflammation (cytokine storm) and shedding (neurodegeneration) of the olfactory neuro-epithelium which interfere with odorants binding to OR[19]

Romantic relationships

edit

Smell plays an important largely nonconscious role in the process of physical and romantic attraction. From an evolutionary perspective, the most important human outcome is successful procreation and the production of healthy offspring capable of procreating themselves. One key factor in evolution is the development genes known as the major histocompatibility complex (MHC). MHC has evolved in humans to allow for individuals to distinguish compatibility efforts through smell when choosing a mate to ensure stronger immune systems for survival in offspring. To test the MHC theory, participants were invited to sniff dirty week-old t-shirts. Participants preferred the odor of the t-shirts that tended to have a genetically different MHC than their own.[20] In ovulating women, it is found that their preference of smell in a male is more reliant on facial attractiveness, suggesting when a woman is at her most fertile smell becomes more crucial in the decision-making process.[21] One way to ensure our offspring will be healthy and effective in this regard is for us to seek, find, and mate with healthy romantic partners. Thus, the process of natural selection has made us particularly attentive to the various signs indicative of the healthiest potential mates we can attract, and one's scent is a particularly important cue to sexual attraction in this regard.[22]

Animal communication

edit
Flehmen response in an African buffalo

In the animal kingdom, chemical communication or semiochemicals is received though the olfactory channel allowing for the animal to decode and recognize chemical signals. Among other animals, animal cognition research finds that dogs are more reliant on olfaction than any other sense.[23] Dogs have a significantly larger olfactory epithelium with 30 percent more olfactory receptors than humans.[24] Having more olfactory receptors that can recognize a much larger variety of odorants. Dogs rely on sniffing to gather past information on their surrounding environment though odor detection and identification allowing them to localize odors. Semiochemicals are "a substance excreted by an animal, to the outside of that individual, which is then received by another individual, classically of the same species, which then elicits some behavioral or developmental response related to the survival of the species."[25]

Many mammals exhibit both odorant and vomeronasal sensitive organs. Pigs communicate through pheromones commonly and also display main olfactory epithelium response to some pheromones. Additionally in mice we see vomeronasal response from odors not produced by animals. Some mutant mice defective in VNO activity continue to display activity that is indicative of pheromone communication. Odorants and pheromones display many similarities. Additionally, it is seen in mice that the response to the presence of certain semiochemicals and odors can be learned. For example, a mouse is not aggressive to the scent of his own pheromones. However, when these pheromones are presented on another male mouse the aggressive behavior will be displayed. Mice form an olfactory memory which helps them define and react to the presence of pheromones. Humans have no VNO but still a level of communication through semiochemicals. These classes of pheromonal action are the opposite-sex attractants, the same-sex repellants (territorial markers), mother–infant bonding attractants and those modulating the timing of the fertile cycle.[26] There is contradictory evidence supporting the hypothesis that humans use pheromones in order to regulate behavior through these four channels.[27] Humans are capable of distinctly recognizing and empathizing with emotion identified from bodily odors.

Research done by Kokocińska-Kusiak A, Woszczyło M, Zybala M, Maciocha J, Barłowska K, Dzięcioł M (August 2021), found that dogs can transmit messages through semiochemicals in urine marking among their environment leaving a "scent mark" for other animals to decode later on. In male dogs and wolves, urine-marking used more frequently in unknown areas and the practice is even continued when there is no urine left, meaning the passing of semiochemicals is not strictly tied to the act of urination.[28] Animal behaviors can be altered by chemical stimuli. Chemical stimuli is a crucial source of information that elicits different behavioral responses from aquatic invertebrates. For example, in Elyssa Rosen's research she was able to find that hermit crabs have different reactions to water that has been previously occupied by a potential predatory and non-predatory crabs. It is important for hermit crabs to be able to decode these chemical stimuli in order to understand if the territory is safe to occupy.[29] The perfect environment to receive semiochemicals is though humidity and light sun. Elements that can negatively disrupt the chemical communication channel is heavy rain in the environment because it brings the scents hanging in the air and burring them into the ground as well as fog that causes a scent to diffuse and linger in the air which can cause confusion.[29]

Emotion

edit
 
Lautenberg Laboratories is the leader in controlled environment beta testing for the personal hygiene and pharmaceutical industry.

Natural human body odors may also carry emotional information that can be decoded by others which allows for communication.[30] In one study, study groups placed cotton gauze into their armpit and were shown fear or happiness inducing videos. Later the sweat samples were given to participants of the opposite sex. They were able to determine the emotional state of the donor at "above chance levels."[30] Researcher Denise Chen and Jeanneette Haviland-Jones's findings the happiness odors can be detected more frequently by women than men.[30] Disgust and fear both have identifiable and distinguishable scents. Should an individual be exposed to the scent of fear (in this case sweat samples from first time skydivers) various parts of the brain are activated: the amygdala and hypothalamus primarily. Additionally, the insula (which processes sensory and emotional information), the fusiform gyrus (which plays a key role in face and object recognition), and the cingulate cortex (which helps regulate responses to pain and emotion).[31] Activating the appropriate regions of the brain when the brain recognizes the presence of applicable semiochemicals allows for more cognition.

Olfactic cues can impact the degree of cooperativeness of an individual.[32] Generally, men are rated as more selfish and less cooperative when presented with a masculine scent. This scent contained higher concentrations of androstadienone. Scent donors were rated on cooperativeness before the sample was taken, and this sample had a positive correlation to the cooperativeness of the subject in a later test. The exact compounds produced in relation to cooperativeness require further investigation.

References

edit
  1. ^ Brill AA (1932-01-01). "The Sense of Smell in the Neuroses and Psychoses". The Psychoanalytic Quarterly. 1 (1): 7–42. doi:10.1080/21674086.1932.11925133. ISSN 0033-2828.
  2. ^ Krusemark EA, Novak LR, Gitelman DR, Li W (September 2013). "When the sense of smell meets emotion: anxiety-state-dependent olfactory processing and neural circuitry adaptation". The Journal of Neuroscience. 33 (39): 15324–15332. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1835-13.2013. PMC 3782615. PMID 24068799.
  3. ^ a b c Neuliep JW (2009). "The Nonverbal Code" (PDF). Intercultural Communication: A Contextual Approach (4th ed.). Los Angeles: Sage. ISBN 978-1-4129-6770-9.
  4. ^ Bontempi C, Jacquot L, Brand G (2021). "Sex Differences in Odor Hedonic Perception: An Overview". Frontiers in Neuroscience. 15: 764520. doi:10.3389/fnins.2021.764520. PMC 8558558. PMID 34733137.
  5. ^ a b Philpott CM, Bennett A, Murty GE (July 2008). "A brief history of olfaction and olfactometry". The Journal of Laryngology and Otology. 122 (7): 657–62. doi:10.1017/S0022215107001314. PMID 18201391. S2CID 20572407.
  6. ^ Synnott A (2008-07-14). "A sociology of smell". Canadian Review of Sociology/Revue Canadienne de Sociologie. 28 (4): 437–459. doi:10.1111/j.1755-618X.1991.tb00164.x.
  7. ^ a b Majid A, Roberts SG, Cilissen L, Emmorey K, Nicodemus B, O'Grady L, et al. (November 2018). "Differential coding of perception in the world's languages". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 115 (45): 11369–11376. Bibcode:2018PNAS..11511369M. doi:10.1073/pnas.1720419115. PMC 6233065. PMID 30397135.
  8. ^ Jraissati Y, Deroy O (January 2021). "Categorizing Smells: A Localist Approach". Cognitive Science. 45 (1): e12930. doi:10.1111/cogs.12930. PMID 33389758. S2CID 230486449.
  9. ^ Majid A (June 2021). "Olfactory Language Requires an Integrative and Interdisciplinary Approach" (PDF). Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 25 (6): 421–422. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2021.03.004. PMID 33757700. S2CID 232288951.
  10. ^ Majid A (February 2021). "Human Olfaction at the Intersection of Language, Culture, and Biology". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 25 (2): 111–123. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2020.11.005. PMID 33349546. S2CID 63437630.
  11. ^ Speed LJ, Majid A (2020). "Grounding language in the neglected senses of touch, taste, and smell". Cognitive Neuropsychology. 37 (5–6): 363–392. doi:10.1080/02643294.2019.1623188. hdl:21.11116/0000-0003-DF6B-B. PMID 31230566. S2CID 195327532.
  12. ^ Sandri A, Cecchini MP, Riello M, Zanini A, Nocini R, Fiorio M, Tinazzi M (June 2021). "Pain, Smell, and Taste in Adults: A Narrative Review of Multisensory Perception and Interaction". Pain and Therapy. 10 (1): 245–268. doi:10.1007/s40122-021-00247-y. PMC 8119564. PMID 33635507.
  13. ^ Schiffman SS (March 2018). "Influence of medications on taste and smell". World Journal of Otorhinolaryngology - Head and Neck Surgery. 4 (1): 84–91. doi:10.1016/j.wjorl.2018.02.005. PMC 6051304. PMID 30035266.
  14. ^ Wrzesniewski A, McCauley C, Rozin P (December 1999). "Odor and affect: individual differences in the impact of odor on liking for places, things and people". Chemical Senses. 24 (6): 713–721. doi:10.1093/chemse/24.6.713. PMID 10587506.
  15. ^ Posner MI, Nissen MJ, Klein RM (March 1976). "Visual dominance: an information-processing account of its origins and significance". Psychological Review. 83 (2): 157–71. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.83.2.157. PMID 769017.
  16. ^ Seo HS, Hummel T (March 2011). "Auditory-olfactory integration: congruent or pleasant sounds amplify odor pleasantness". Chemical Senses. 36 (3): 301–309. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjq129. PMID 21163913.
  17. ^ "Your Nose (for Kids) - Nemours KidsHealth". kidshealth.org. Retrieved 2022-11-23.
  18. ^ Mullol J, Alobid I, Mariño-Sánchez F, Izquierdo-Domínguez A, Marin C, Klimek L, et al. (August 2020). "The Loss of Smell and Taste in the COVID-19 Outbreak: a Tale of Many Countries". Current Allergy and Asthma Reports. 20 (10): 61. doi:10.1007/s11882-020-00961-1. PMC 7397453. PMID 32748211.
  19. ^ Butowt R, Bilinska K (May 2020). "SARS-CoV-2: Olfaction, Brain Infection, and the Urgent Need for Clinical Samples Allowing Earlier Virus Detection". ACS Chemical Neuroscience. 11 (9): 1200–1203. doi:10.1021/acschemneuro.0c00172. PMC 7160911. PMID 32283006.
  20. ^ Wedekind C, Füri S (October 1997). "Body odour preferences in men and women: do they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygosity?". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 264 (1387): 1471–1479. doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0204. PMC 1688704. PMID 9364787.
  21. ^ Gangestad SW, Thornhill R (May 1998). "Menstrual cycle variation in women's preferences for the scent of symmetrical men". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 265 (1399): 927–933. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0380. PMC 1689051. PMID 9633114.
  22. ^ Burgoon JK (2021). Nonverbal communication. Valerie Lynn Manusov, Laura K. Guerrero (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge. p. 68. ISBN 978-1-003-09555-2. OCLC 1240266780.
  23. ^ Horowitz A, Franks B (January 2020). "What smells? Gauging attention to olfaction in canine cognition research". Animal Cognition. 23 (1): 11–18. doi:10.1007/s10071-019-01311-z. PMID 31606800. S2CID 254139189.
  24. ^ Jenkins EK, DeChant MT, Perry EB (2018-03-29). "When the Nose Doesn't Know: Canine Olfactory Function Associated With Health, Management, and Potential Links to Microbiota". Frontiers in Veterinary Science. 5: 56. doi:10.3389/fvets.2018.00056. PMC 5884888. PMID 29651421.
  25. ^ Karlson P, Luscher M (January 1959). "Pheromones': a new term for a class of biologically active substances". Nature. 183 (4653): 55–56. Bibcode:1959Natur.183...55K. doi:10.1038/183055a0. PMID 13622694. S2CID 4243699.
  26. ^ McCoy NL, Pitino L (March 2002). "Pheromonal influences on sociosexual behavior in young women". Physiology & Behavior. 75 (3): 367–75. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.738.6372. doi:10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00675-8. PMID 11897264. S2CID 14686598.
  27. ^ Winman A (September 2004). "Do perfume additives termed human pheromones warrant being termed pheromones?". Physiology & Behavior. 82 (4): 697–701. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.06.006. PMID 15327919. S2CID 38252501.
  28. ^ Kokocińska-Kusiak A, Woszczyło M, Zybala M, Maciocha J, Barłowska K, Dzięcioł M (August 2021). "Canine Olfaction: Physiology, Behavior, and Possibilities for Practical Applications". Animals. 11 (8): 2463. doi:10.3390/ani11082463. PMC 8388720. PMID 34438920.
  29. ^ a b Rosen E, Schwarz B, Palmer AR (September 2009). "Smelling the difference: hermit crab responses to predatory and nonpredatory crabs". Animal Behaviour. 78 (3): 691–695. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.05.035. ISSN 0003-3472. S2CID 36883299.
  30. ^ a b c Chen D, Haviland-Jones J (December 2000). "Human olfactory communication of emotion". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 91 (3 Pt 1): 771–781. doi:10.2466/pms.2000.91.3.771. PMID 11153847. S2CID 1086223.
  31. ^ Prehn-Kristensen A, Wiesner C, Bergmann TO, Wolff S, Jansen O, Mehdorn HM, et al. (June 2009). "Induction of empathy by the smell of anxiety". PLOS ONE. 4 (6): e5987. Bibcode:2009PLoSO...4.5987P. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0005987. PMC 2695008. PMID 19551135.
  32. ^ Tognetti A, Durand V, Dubois D, Barkat-Defradas M, Hopfensitz A, Ferdenzi C (May 2022). "The smell of cooperativeness: Do human body odours advertise cooperative behaviours?" (PDF). British Journal of Psychology. 113 (2): 531–546. doi:10.1111/bjop.12544. PMID 34882779. S2CID 245013664.