Social intelligence

(Redirected from Social Intelligence)

Social intelligence is the ability to understand one's own and others' actions. Social intelligence is learned and develops from experience with people and learning from success and failures in social settings. It is an important interpersonal skill that helps individuals succeed in all aspects of their lives.[1]

Definitions

edit

The original definition of social intelligence (by Edward Thorndike in 1920) is "the ability to understand and manage men and women and boys and girls, to act wisely in human relations".[2] It is thus equivalent to interpersonal intelligence, one of the types of intelligence identified in Howard Gardner's theory of multiple intelligences, and closely related to theory of mind.[citation needed]

Social scientist Ross Honeywill postulates that social intelligence is an aggregated measure of self- and social-awareness, evolved social beliefs and attitudes, and a capacity and appetite to manage complex social change.[citation needed] Neuropsychologist Nicholas Humphrey believes that social intelligence defines who we are as humans.[3]

An updated definition coined by Nancy Cantor and John F. Kihlstrom in 1987 is “the individual's fund of knowledge about the social world."[4] In 2006 Eleni Andreou described social intelligence as being similar to "social skills and competence".[5]

Social intelligence and interpersonal intelligence were previously believed to be closely related, however, the subjects diverged into two distinct fields of study.[6]

Other authors restricted the definition to concern only knowledge of social situations, perhaps more properly called social cognition (or social marketing intelligence, as it pertains to trending socio-psychological advertising and marketing strategies and tactics). According to Sean Foleno, social intelligence is a person's competence to optimally understand one's environment and react appropriately for socially successful conduct.[7]

These multiple definitions demonstrate a lack of consensus on the operational definition of social intelligence.[8]

In psychology, Social Intelligence is a critical subset of human intelligence centered around two core components: social awareness and social facility.

Social cognition refers to the capacity to understand and empathize with others’ emotions and perspectives, while social facility pertains to the ability to behave effectively in social situations.

Hypothesis

edit

The social intelligence hypothesis states that social intelligence (that is, complex socialization such as politics, romance, family relationships, quarrels, collaboration, reciprocity, and altruism)

  1. was a driving force in developing the size of human brains or "executive brains"
  2. today provides our ability to use those large brains in complex social circumstances.[9]

This hypothesis claims that the demands of living together drives our need for intelligence, and that social intelligence is an evolutionary adaptation for dealing with highly complex social situations, as well as for gaining and maintaining power in social groups.

Archaeologist Steve Mithen believes that there are two key evolutionary periods of human brain growth that contextualize the social intelligence hypothesis. The first was about two million years ago, when the brain more than doubled in size. Mithen believes that this growth was because people were living in larger, more complex groups, and had to keep track of more people and relationships. These changes required a greater mental capacity and, in turn, a larger brain size.[citation needed]

The second key growth period in human brain size occurred between 600,000 and 200,000 years ago, when the brain reached its modern size. While this growth is still not fully explained, Mithen believes that it is related to the evolution of language. Language may be the most complex cognitive task we undertake.[10][citation needed] Language is related to social intelligence because it is used to mediate social relationships.[11][12]

Social intelligence was a critical factor in brain growth. Social and cognitive complexity co-evolve.[13][page needed]

Measurement

edit

The social intelligence quotient (SQ) is a statistical abstraction, similar to the ‘standard score’ approach used in IQ tests, with a mean of 100. Scores of 140 or above are considered to be very high. Unlike the standard IQ test, it is not a fixed model.[14][better source needed] It leans more toward Jean Piaget's theory that intelligence is not a fixed attribute, but a complex hierarchy of information-processing skills underlying an adaptive equilibrium between the individual and the environment.[15] Therefore, an individual can change their SQ by altering their attitudes and behavior in response to their social environment.[14]

SQ had been measured by techniques such as question and answer sessions. These sessions assessed the person's pragmatic abilities to test eligibility in certain special education courses[clarification needed]; however, some tests have been developed to measure social intelligence. This test[specify] can be used to diagnose autism spectrum disorders. This test[specify] can also be used to check for some non-autistic or semi-autistic conditions such as social communication disorder or SCD, schizophrenia, dyssemia, and ADHD.[citation needed]

Some self-report social intelligence measures exist.[8] Although easy to administer, the resulting social intelligence measures might better be interpreted in terms of social self-efficacy (that is, one's confidence in one's ability to deal with social information).[16]

Since people with low SQ scores may not have skills necessary to communicate with customers and/or co-workers, they are most successful with minimal customer interaction, smaller groups, or independent work. People with SQs over 120 are considered socially skilled, and may work exceptionally well in jobs that involve direct contact and communication with other people.[citation needed]

The George Washington University Social Intelligence Test is one of the only ability measures available for assessing social intelligence. It was created in June 1928 by Dr. Thelma Hunt, a psychologist from George Washington University.[17] It was originally proposed as a measurement of a person's capacity to deal with people and social relationships.[18] The test assesses social abilities like observing human behavior, social situation judgement, name & face memory, and theory of mind from facial expressions.[17] The George Washington University Social Intelligence Test revised second edition consists of these items:[18]

  • Observation of human behavior
  • Recognition of the mental state of the speaker
  • Memory for names and faces
  • Judgment in social situations
  • Sense of humor

Differences from intelligence

edit

Nicholas Humphrey points to a difference between intelligence being measured by IQ tests and social intelligence.[citation needed] Some autistic children are extremely intelligent because they have well developed skills of observing and memorizing information, however they have low social intelligence. For a long time, the field[specify] was dominated by behaviorism, that is, the theory that one could understand animals, including humans, just by observing their behavior and finding correlations. But subsequent theories argue that one must consider the inner structure behavior[clarification needed].[19]

Additional views

edit

Social intelligence is closely related to cognition and emotional intelligence.[citation needed] Research psychologists studying social cognition and social neuroscience have discovered many principles in which human social intelligence operates. Psychologists Nancy Cantor and John Kihlstrom outlined the kinds of concepts people use to make sense of their social relations (e.g., "What situation am I in?, What kind of person is this?, Who is talking to me?"), the rules they use to draw inferences ("What did he mean by that?") and plan actions ("What am I going to do about it?").[20]

More recently, popular science writer Daniel Goleman has drawn on social neuroscience research to propose that social intelligence is made up of social awareness (including empathy, attunement, empathic accuracy, and social cognition) and social facility (including synchrony, self-presentation, influence, and concern).[21] Goleman's research indicates that our social relationships effect our physical health, and the deeper the relationship the deeper the impact.[further explanation needed] Effects include blood flow[specify], breathing[specify], mood such as fatigue and depression, and weakening of the immune system.[21]

Researcher Raymond H. Hartjen believes that intelligence is amplified by increased social interactions.[citation needed] This suggests that children require continuous opportunities for interpersonal experiences in order to develop a keen 'inter-personal psychology'.[citation needed] Traditional classrooms do not permit the interaction of complex social behavior[clarification needed]. Instead, students in traditional settings are treated as learners who must be infused with more and more complex forms of information. The structure of schools today allows very few of these skills[specify], critical for survival in the world, to develop. Because we so limit the development of the skills of "natural psychologist" in traditional schools, graduates enter the job market handicapped to the point of being incapable of surviving on their own.[citation needed] In contrast, students who have had an opportunity to develop their skills in multi-age classrooms with democratic settings rise above their less socially skilled peers. They have a good sense of self, know what they want in life and have the skills to begin their quest.[22]

J. P. Guilford was the first researcher to approach the problem of social intelligence from the measurement viewpoint.[dubiousdiscuss] He developed a test of social intelligence, and suggested that the social intelligence is a unit that does not depend on common intellectual factor, but relates to the comprehension of behavioral information.[23]

See also

edit

References

edit
  1. ^ Adler, Ronald (2022). Communicating at Work (13th ed.). United States: McGraw-Hill Higher Education. p. 126. ISBN 9781265055738.
  2. ^ Thorndike, Edward Lee (1 January 1920). "Intelligence and its uses". Harper's Magazine.
  3. ^ Humphrey, Nicholas (1976). Growing Points in Ethology. Cambridge University Press. pp. 303–317.
  4. ^ Kihlstrom, John F.; Cantor, Nancy (2000), "Social Intelligence", in Sternberg, Robert J. (ed.), Handbook of Intelligence, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 359–379, doi:10.1017/cbo9780511807947.017, ISBN 978-0-521-59648-0, retrieved 2022-10-14
  5. ^ Andreou, Eleni (2006). "Social Preference, Perceived Popularity and Social Intelligence: Relations to Overt and Relational Aggression". School Psychology International. 27 (3): 339–351. doi:10.1177/0143034306067286. ISSN 0143-0343. S2CID 142982473 – via Sage Journals.
  6. ^ Walker, Ronald E.; Foley, Jeanne M. (1973). "Social Intelligence: Its History and Measurement". Psychological Reports. 33 (3): 839–864. doi:10.2466/pr0.1973.33.3.839. ISSN 0033-2941. S2CID 144839425 – via Sage Journals.
  7. ^ M, Manasa Prasad; Dsouza, Dr Greta (9 October 2019). "Need of Social Intelligence for Future Teachers". Journal of the Gujarat Research Society. 21 (3): 261–265. ISSN 0374-8588. Retrieved 5 June 2024.
  8. ^ a b Grieve, Rachel; Mahar, Doug (1 March 2013). "Can social intelligence be measured? Psychometric properties of the Tromsø Social Intelligence Scale – English Version". The Irish Journal of Psychology. 34 (1): 1–12. doi:10.1080/03033910.2012.737758.
  9. ^ Holekamp, Kay E. (2007). "Questioning the social intelligence hypothesis". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 11 (2): 65–69. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.003. ISSN 1364-6613. PMID 17188553. S2CID 18654268.
  10. ^ Federmeier, Kara D.; Jongman, Suzanne R.; Szewczyk, Jakub M. (December 2020). "Examining the Role of General Cognitive Skills in Language Processing: A Window Into Complex Cognition". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 29 (6): 575–582. doi:10.1177/0963721420964095. PMC 7877800.
  11. ^ Leonard, Melinda A.; Milich, Richard; Lorch, Elizabeth P. (April 2011). "The Role of Pragmatic Language Use in Mediating the Relation Between Hyperactivity and Inattention and Social Skills Problems". Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 54 (2): 567–579. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0058). PMID 20719870.
  12. ^ Agha, Asif (16 November 2006). Language and Social Relations. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511618284. ISBN 978-0-521-57176-0.
  13. ^ Hardy-Vallée, Benoit (2008). The Philosophy of Social Cognition.
  14. ^ a b "Social Intelligence Lab". Social Intelligence Lab. Retrieved 2012-09-29.[unreliable source?]
  15. ^ "Piaget's developmental theory". Learningandteaching.info. 2010-02-10. Archived from the original on 2012-11-03. Retrieved 2012-09-29.
  16. ^ Grieve, Rachel; Witteveen, Kate; Tolan, G. Anne; Jacobson, Brett (1 March 2014). "Development and validation of a measure of cognitive and behavioural social self-efficacy". Personality and Individual Differences. 59: 71–76. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.11.008.
  17. ^ a b Hunt, Thelma (1928). "The measurement of social intelligence". Journal of Applied Psychology. 12 (3): 317–334. doi:10.1037/h0075832.
  18. ^ a b Hunt, T.; Moss, F. A.; Omwake, K. T.; Woodward, L. G. (1955), "George Washington University Social Intelligence Test – Revised Form, 2nd Edition (GWSIT)", Psychology Resources Centre, York University
  19. ^ Honeywill, Ross (2015). The Man Problem: destructive masculinity in Western culture. New York: Palgrave Macmillan. Social intelligence is also being able to make important social decisions which can change your life
  20. ^ Kihlstron, John F.; Cantor, Nancy. "Social Intelligence". John F. Kihlstrom. Archived from the original on 2015-12-22. Retrieved 2015-12-20.
  21. ^ a b Goleman, Daniel (2006). Social Intelligence: The New Science of Human Relationships. Bantam Books. ISBN 978-0-553-80352-5.
  22. ^ Hartjen, Raymond H. "The Preeminent Intelligence – Social IQ". Beyond the Cutting Edge. Archived from the original on 2010-05-04. Retrieved March 5, 2010.
  23. ^ Guilford, J. P. (1967). The Nature of Human Intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill. p. 538.
edit