A pack is a social group of conspecific canines. The number of members in a pack and their social behavior varies from species to species. Social structure is very important in a pack. Canine packs are led by a breeding pair, consisting of the alpha male and alpha female.[citation needed]
Pack behaviour in specific species
editAfrican wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) live and hunt in packs. Males assist in raising the pups, and stay with their pack for life. The females leave their birth pack at approximately 2.5 years old to join another pack without females. Males outnumber the females in a pack. Typically, only one female is present to breed with all males. African wild dogs are not territorial, and hunt cooperatively in their packs. For example, they will run down large game and tear it apart with their pack. They also cooperate in caring for wounded, sick, and young pack members.[1]
Gray wolves (Canis lupus) tend to live in packs that consist of adult parents and their offspring of the last two or three years. The adult parents are usually unrelated, and other unrelated wolves may sometimes join the pack.[2] Wolves usually hunt in packs, but they hunt alone in the spring and summer months when plenty of prey is available.[citation needed] They are found in both Eurasia and North America.
Black-backed jackals (Canis mesomelas) in Southern and Eastern Africa and coyotes (Canis latrans) in North America have a single long-term mate. As such, they usually hunt alone or in pairs.[3][4] A pack consists of the breeding pair and their current young. They occasionally cooperate in larger packs to hunt big game, but rarely hunt animals larger than a small, young antelope. Black-backed jackals are not typically considered 'aggressive' towards larger animals but tend to be wary of humans. However, they will become aggressive if they feel threatened, in order to defend the boundaries of their territories.
The Ethiopian wolf (Canis simensis) pack members hunt for rodents alone, and come together mainly to defend their territory from rival packs.[5]
Corsac foxes sometimes form packs, unlike some other fox species.[6]
Pack behaviour in gray wolves
editThis section's factual accuracy is disputed. (July 2019) |
Wolves are recognized for creating cooperative relationships within their pack structure, which significantly influence their interactions with one another,[7] perhaps as a reflection of the relationships they present in the pack, reflecting any tension, cooperation, and competition present.[7] Tensions are noted to become higher around breeding season. This is due to increased opportunities for reproduction. Individuals challenge the group hierarchy to gain better-quality mates. This leads to greater intraspecific competition and rising tensions.[7] Female wolves are known for being the main initiators of affiliative interactions, though a small percentage of males will initiate affiliative interactions.[7] The omega male is not a target of any affiliative interaction.[7] In other studies, researchers have separated the most dominant wolf from the most subordinate wolves.[8] It was recorded that the dominant wolf spent less time sleeping and showed more behavioural stress compared to the omega wolf.[8] The dominant wolf was reported to rest in the section of his enclosure closest to his pack.[8] Furthermore, researchers noted that younger, more subordinate wolves appear to have less attachment to their pack compared to their higher-ranking compatriots.[8]
Cooperation is essential for tasks such as hunting and protecting the young, though the level of attachment present in the pack is not necessarily equal.[8] The majority of wolves are known for dispersing from their birth pack; this makes measuring attachment behavior within the packs difficult.[9] There are cases in which wolves leave their pack, typically when accompanying siblings of the same sex.[9] This behaviour is suggested to be adaptive, which will benefit pack mates in future conflicts.[9]
All individuals benefit from being a member of the wolf pack; the weak are supported by the efforts of stronger wolves,[10][11] and higher-ranking individuals enjoy better and larger kills than could be taken on their own.[12] Protection is granted by sheer number, and larger, more plentiful territory can be won and sustained.[13] Care and protection of the young are shared, and knowledge can be passed down through generations, creating a unique culture within each group[10]
The pack is typically a nuclear family unit.[14] It often consists of 5–10 (though in areas of high prey abundance can be up to 30)[15] mostly related individuals, specifically consisting of a typically unrelated breeding pair also known as the alphas,[16] their offspring, and occasionally a handful of other wolves which can be related or not.[14] Membership may be fluid and is subject to change.[17] Outside wolves may be shunned or, more rarely, accepted, depending on the specific circumstances. Genetic variability can become limited within such an interrelated group, and so conditions for gene flow must exist.[16] Outside wolves can provide these opportunities.[10] A pack may accept another wolf into their group if it is a distant relative,[14] if reproduction rates are low due to the loss or infertility of an alpha, or if their numbers are significantly reduced.[16][failed verification]
Characterisation of wolves into dominance hierarchies of alpha, beta, and omega was based on behavioural studies of unrelated wolves in captivity, and this assemblage largely does not apply to natural wolf packs, which are familial units.[14]
Wolves without packs
editThese singular outside wolves, often referred to as lone wolves, are vulnerable to food scarcity and territorial attacks and generally comprise less than 15% of the total wolf population. Lone wolves usually result from sexually mature offspring leaving their parental pack, though may also occur if harassed subordinates chose to disperse. In times of prey scarcity, low-ranking wolves may choose to go off on their own if the pack cannot supply sufficient food. These lone wolves may then attempt to join an existing wolf pack or, more commonly, find a mate and begin a new pack family as the alphas.[16][obsolete source]
The breeding pair ("alphas")
editWithin the wolf pack, the breeding pair or the dominant breeding pair (in packs with multiple breeders), often referred to in familiar language as the "alpha pair" or the "alpha wolves", are typically the members of the family unit which breed and produce offspring; they are the matriarch and patriarch of the family.[15] It was previously believed to be common for an aging or sick alpha to be replaced by one of their offspring,[16] but more recent studies have shown this incestuous behaviour to be very rare.[18]
The pups
editThe importance of the alpha is rivalled only by that of the pups.[19][better source needed] The fundamental purpose of the pack is the successful production of offspring, and so raising the litter is a collaborative venture – all members contribute to their development.[10] In times of scarcity, the breeding pair will often prioritise the care of the pups and preferentially feed the youngest wolves first.[14] Despite this committed involvement, pup mortality is high, with researchers citing that only roughly 30% survive their first year of life.[20] Those who survive, however, grow up with the added advantage of being surrounded by numerous caretakers and teachers. There exists a culture within wolf packs, and this is passed on to the offspring by the elders of the group. Pups learn something from each member of the pack and attain the vital social skills required to create powerful bonds upon which the wolf's societal structure relies.[10]
Dominance and the "alpha wolf"
editAnimals that typically predominate over others are associated with the term alpha. Among pack-living wolves, alpha wolves are the genetic parents of most cubs in the pack. Such access to mating females creates strong selective pressure for intra-sex competition.[21]
Wolves show deference to the alpha pair in their pack by allowing them to allocate the distribution of food, typically preferentially feeding the youngest wolves. Wolves use eye contact and posture as an indicator of dominance or submission, which are largely age-based; these postures are rare except concerning food, as described previously. The smaller and more nuclear a pack is, the less status of alpha is likely to be obtained through fighting, and young wolves instead leave the pack to find a mate and produce offspring of their own.[14] Larger or less-nuclear packs may operate differently and possess more complex and flexible social structures.[22][better source needed]
In the case of other wild canids, the alpha male may not have exclusive access to the alpha female;[23]: 502 [obsolete source] moreover, other pack members may guard the maternity den used by the alpha female; as with the African wild dog, Lycaon pictus.[24][full citation needed]
As dominant roles may be deemed normal among social species with extended parenting, it has been suggested that the additional term alpha is not required merely to describe dominance due to its ubiquity but should be reserved for where they are the predominant pack progenitor. For instance, wolf biologist L. David Mech stated:[14]
...calling a wolf an alpha is usually no more appropriate than referring to a human parent or a doe deer as an alpha. Any parent is dominant to its young offspring, so alpha adds no information. Why not refer to an alpha female as the female parent, the breeding female, the matriarch, or simply the mother? Such a designation emphasizes not the animal's dominant status, which is trivial information, but its role as pack progenitor, which is critical information. The one use we may still want to reserve for alpha is in the relatively few large wolf packs comprised of multiple litters. ... In such cases, the older breeders are probably dominant to the younger breeders and perhaps can more appropriately be called the alphas. ... The point here is not so much the terminology but what the terminology falsely implies: a rigid, force-based dominance hierarchy.
Use in dog training
editOne of the most persistent but disputed[25] theories in dog training literature is the idea of the alpha wolf, an individual gray wolf who uses body language and, when needed, physical force to maintain dominance within the wolf pack. The idea was first reported in early wolf research.[2] It was subsequently adopted by dog trainers.[26] Later research has disputed the theory, pointing out that it was based on the behaviour of captive packs consisting of unrelated individuals, while in nature a pack is usually made up of members of a family.[27]
The term alpha was popularised as early as 1976 in the dog training book How to Be Your Dog's Best Friend (Monks of New Skete), which introduced the idea of the alpha roll, a technique for punishing unwanted dog behaviours. Psychologist and dog trainer Stanley Coren wrote in the 2001 book How to Speak Dog, "You are the alpha dog... You must communicate that you are the pack leader and dominant".[28]
It has been suggested that the use of such techniques may have more to do with human psychology than with dog behavior; "dominance hierarchies and dominance disputes and testing are a fundamental characteristic of all social groups... But perhaps only we humans learn to use punishment primarily to gain for ourselves the reward of being dominant.[29]
Most leading veterinary and animal behaviour associations and most contemporary trainers would agree to advocate the use of rewards to teach commands and encourage good communication between owners and their pets. Many modern practices dictate the abandonment of outdated "pack" methods.[30] Some canine behaviourists suggest that kind, efficient training uses games to teach commands which can be utilised to benefit the owner's everyday life.[31]
See also
editReferences
edit- ^ Animal Diversity Web. "Lycaon pictus: Information". University of Michigan. Retrieved 21 April 2008.
- ^ a b L. David Mech PP. "Schenkel's Classic Wolf Behavior Study Available in English". Archived from the original on 10 August 2013. Retrieved 21 April 2008.
...Schenkel's 1947 "Expressions Studies on Wolves", the study that gave rise to the now outmoded notion of alpha wolves. That concept was based on the old idea that wolves fight within a pack to gain dominance and that the winner is the "alpha" wolf. Today we understand that most wolf packs consist of a pair of adults called "parents" or "breeders," and their offspring.
- ^ Animal Diversity Web. "Canis mesomelas: Information". University of Michigan. Retrieved 21 April 2008.
- ^ "Coyotes 101 – CoyoteSmart". www.coyotesmarts.org. Retrieved 14 April 2018.
- ^ "Ethiopian Wolf". Animal Info. 2005-03-07. Retrieved 2015-02-26.
- ^ Clark, Howard O.; Murdoch, James D. Jr.; Newman, Darren P.; Sillero-Zubiri, Claudio (2009). "'Vulpes corsac' (Carnivora: Canidae)". Mammalian Species. 832: 1–8. doi:10.1644/832.1.
- ^ a b c d e Cipponeri, Traci; Verrell, Paul (April 2003). "An uneasy alliance: unequal distribution of affiliate interactions among members of a captive wolf pack". Canadian Journal of Zoology. 81 (10): 1763–1766. doi:10.1139/z03-159.
- ^ a b c d e Fox, M.W (1973). "Social Dynamic of Three Captive Wolf Packs". Behaviour. 47 (3–4): 290–301. doi:10.1163/156853973x00139.
- ^ a b c Cassidy, Kira; McIntyre, Richard T. (May 2016). "Do gray wolves ('Canis lupus') support pack mates during aggressive inter-pack interactions?". Animal Cognition. 19 (5): 939–947. doi:10.1007/s10071-016-0994-1. PMID 27193460. S2CID 3894642.
- ^ a b c d e "The Social Wolf". Living with Wolves. Retrieved February 20, 2019.
- ^ "The wolf and the wolf pack: a perfect mechanism of social organisation". blog.almonature.com. 2015-07-07. Retrieved 2023-09-21.
- ^ Schmidt, Paul A.; Mech, L. David (October 1997). "Wolf Pack Size And Food Acquisition". The American Naturalist. 150 (4): 513–517. doi:10.1086/286079. PMID 18811290. S2CID 3175156.
- ^ "Pack power". The Economist. March 30, 2015. Archived from the original on 1 January 2021.
- ^ a b c d e f g Mech, L David (1 November 1999). "Alpha status, dominance, and division of labor in wolf packs". Canadian Journal of Zoology. 77 (8): 1196–1203. doi:10.1139/z99-099. Archived from the original on 2004-12-21. Retrieved March 17, 2019.
The point here is not so much the terminology but what the terminology falsely implies: a rigid, force-based dominance hierarchy.
PDF available at wolf.org - ^ a b "Gray Wolf Biology & Behavior". Western Wildlife Outreach. Retrieved February 24, 2019.
- ^ a b c d e "Wolf Pack Society". www.tamonganaturepark.com. Tamonga Nature Park. Archived from the original on 25 April 2019. Retrieved February 24, 2019.[obsolete source]
- ^ "Wolf FAQs & Answers". International Wolf Center. 17 May 2013. Retrieved March 2, 2019.
- ^ Smith, Deborah; Meier, Thomas; Geffen, Eli; Mech, L. David; et al. (1 July 1997). "Is incest common in gray wolf packs?". Behavioral Ecology. 8 (4): 384–391. doi:10.1093/beheco/8.4.384.
- ^ Ellis, Shaun; Junor, Penny (2009). The man who lives with wolves (First ed.). London: HarperCollins. ISBN 978-0307464705.
- ^ Smith, Doug (March 5, 2015). "Wolf pup survival a fragile thing". Star Tribune. Archived from the original on 27 August 2021. Retrieved March 27, 2019.
- ^ Sillero-Zubiri; Gottelli; MacDonald (1996). "Male Philopatry, Extra-Pack Copulations and Inbreeding Avoidance in Ethiopian Wolves (Canis simensis)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 38 (5): 331–340. doi:10.1007/s002650050249. S2CID 43709256 – via JSTOR.
- ^ Dutcher, Jim; Dutcher, Jamie (2002). Wolves at our door. New York: Simon & Schuster; Pocket Books. ISBN 978-0743400480.
- ^ Klinghammer, Erich (1998). "Wolves". In Greenberg, Gary; Haraway, Maury M. (eds.). Comparative psychology: A handbook. New York: Routledge. pp. 499–506. ISBN 978-0815312819.[obsolete source]
- ^ C. Michael Hogan. 2009[full citation needed]
- ^ Miklósi, Ádám (2018). The Dog: A Natural History. Princeton University Press. p. 75. ISBN 9780691176932.
- ^ "ClickerSolutions Training Articles - The History and Misconceptions of Dominance Theory". Clickersolutions.com. Retrieved 2015-02-26.
- ^ Mech, L. David. (1999). "Alpha status, dominance, and division of labor in wolf packs". Canadian Journal of Zoology. 77 (8): 1196–1203. doi:10.1139/z99-099. Archived from the original on 14 December 2005.
- ^ Coren, Stanley (2001). "Chapter 20". How to Speak Dog (First ed.). Simon & Schuster; Fireside. p. 250. ISBN 0-684-86534-3.
- ^ Pryor, Karen (August 1999). "Chapter 4". Don't Shoot the Dog! (Bantam trade paperback ed.). Bantam Books. p. 108. ISBN 0-553-38039-7.
- ^ "Position Statement on the Use of Dominance Theory in Behavior Modification of Animals" (PDF). American Veterinary Society of Animal Behavior. Retrieved 17 February 2021.
- ^ "Dog behaviour pack leaders debunked". Affinity Dog Training. Retrieved 2013-08-16.