Review waiting, please be patient.
This may take 7 weeks or more, since drafts are reviewed in no specific order. There are 1,552 pending submissions waiting for review.
Where to get help
How to improve a draft
You can also browse Wikipedia:Featured articles and Wikipedia:Good articles to find examples of Wikipedia's best writing on topics similar to your proposed article. Improving your odds of a speedy review To improve your odds of a faster review, tag your draft with relevant WikiProject tags using the button below. This will let reviewers know a new draft has been submitted in their area of interest. For instance, if you wrote about a female astronomer, you would want to add the Biography, Astronomy, and Women scientists tags. Editor resources
Reviewer tools
|
Social schema theory is based on social and cognative theories, emphasising how information is received, stored, recalled and edited due to constant environmental interaction. This theory uses the cognitive term schema to mean a mental representation of a concept [1] and focuses on its interactional influence regarding human behaviour.
As such, social schema theory [2] identifies four main types of schemas. Self-schemas store information relating to the person’s identity, Person schemas organise information pertinent to the social groups and assumptions associated with strangers, Event schemas predominantly use information from past experiences to produce appropriate behaviour, whereas Role schemas interpret information based on societal norms.
The formation of social schemas is automatic and unconsciously based on past and current contexts. Importantly, social schemas help to interpret the ever-changing and fast-paced social world, enabling humans to interact with the external environment without having to manually think through each situation. As a result, social schema theory has a wide social application despite its theory being based heavily on theoretical background. Some gaps are left in measuring and operationalising the formation of social schemas and their subtypes.
What is a schema?
editThe term schema can be traced back to the Greek word “schéma” and refers to a theoretical mental framework or heuristic used to interpret incoming information and direct behaviour [3]. Depending on the nature of the incoming information, schemas can be further broken down into four subsections [4]. Once this information is organised into the correct schema subsection, stored previous information may be altered to maintain an accurate representation of an event or person. Subsequently, schemas are imperative for learning and development as this flexible approach allows for the reinterpretation of experience as more knowledge is acquired[5].
Furthermore, schemas are then used to recall information when faced with similar situations to facilitate the production of appropriate behaviours [6]. Concisely, this process allows for socially related knowledge to be organised, interpreted, edited if necessary and recalled in a short space of time. This theory has had several practical applications, such as schema therapy in the context of love trauma syndrome[7], in an attempt to reduce distressing symptoms after the conclusion of a romantic relationship.
Limitations of schemas
editCritically, among research into the application of social schemas in treatments or therapy, a consistently small and uniform sample was tested[8], preventing any onward generalizability of findings to other social groups. Furthermore, alternative research suggests that schema therapy focuses too much on immediate change, resulting in little focus on symptom relief[7]. As a result, practical internal applications of social schemas are influential in the interpretation of highly predictable situations where as the therapeutic application of social schemas requires more consideration before being prescribed.
Self schemas
editThe term self-schema is used to denote how an individual views themselves morally and characteristically. The concept of the self in the self-schema can be represented in many different ways, from perceptions about one’s own body image and appearance to understanding one’s own personality, ethical standards and preferences[9]. Understanding our self-schemas can assist us in comprehending the motivations behind our actions and changing them accordingly. We perceive ourselves as having a cause-and-effect relationship with the environment, and as such, our self-schemas are likely to influence our behaviour[10]. These self-schemas adapt and evolve over time, allowing for personal growth and development whilst building on previous experiences of similar or new situations[11].
Furthering this concept, how we perceive ourselves will affect how we view other people. If an individual and their self-schema strongly hold onto a specific characteristic, this feature is also likely to be salient when evaluating other people and places. An example of the prevalence and importance of a self-schema can be demonstrated when reviewing academic performance among children[12]. Those whose self-schema represents interest and value in education and learning in a specific subject are more likely to participate and actively engage in academic tasks.
However, research findings to this effect lack empirical causal evidence to support its hypothesis[13], instead providing correlational relationships that cannot measure or quantify the construction or application of a self-schema besides suggesting that one is present. As such, the attribution of self-schemas is largely dependent on anecdotal evidence that could be a result of circumstantial or confounding variables. In summary, self-schemas are required to gain valuable insight into one’s own motivations and preferences when behaving but are, however, not currently quantifiable.
Person schemas
editPerson schemas are developed and used when meeting and interacting with other people. Person schemas draw on relevant and available information when meeting a stranger and combine it with previous knowledge to categorise the individual into face-value social groups. Conversely, when interacting with friends or family members, these schemas hold a prescribed list of personal characteristics and social groups to which they belong, assisting in faster and more socially accurate communication[14].
Person schemas can, however, be wrongly applied to individuals based on inherent bias or outdated and stereotypical schemas. As such, when approaching a new individual, one might judge someone by their appearance [15], the intonation or language used when speaking [16] or the social groups we assume that others around them belong to. From observing these factors, schemas will determine what social group we deem them to belong to, thus creating a shortcut for the next time we interact with them.
These schemas are required when interacting with strangers and family and can cause distress when not appropriately utilised or momentarily forgotten. An example of a person’s schema being disturbed is Capgras delusion [17]. This is when an individual believes that a close family member or friend has been removed and replaced by a stranger or imposter. This misidentification disorder displays the importance of person schemas, as the individuals themselves are intact, but the interpretation of their qualities is disorganised and, therefore, not applied, resulting in distress and confusion.
Whilst this research provides evidential support for social scheme theory, it is important for human cognition not to be oversimplified. Alternative research indicates that too much emphasis is attributed to schemas, limiting investigations into the underlying cognitive factors that integrate incoming information and coordinate between other processes, such as perception and memory. This theory also has explanatory power in real-world applications in attributing characteristics to strangers when first meeting them. However, it is unclear if social schema theory holds the only explanatory power for such conditions[18].
Event schemas
editSchemas hold general information relative to a subject or event. These schemas organise and suggest a broad principle for an action, then used in context as a mental shortcut to act appropriately in the moment [19]. An event schema helps in the recollection of abstract or situational models of events by drawing on past experiences of similar situations [20][21].
These event schemas are flexible and are based upon a large bank of continuously evolving information from the environment. For example, the process of entering and ordering at a restaurant is largely the same regardless of the establishment. It is largely accepted that you wait by a stand until a server takes you to your table. These similar events are combined into event schemas in which this process becomes easily activated and automatic. An accurate representation of an event is now formed from this schema. Event schemas are reactivated when engaging in similar situations and help to dictate appropriate behaviour [22].
One limitation of an event schema is the tendency to construct and fill in any gaps of missing information in an ambiguous situation with assumptions from previous similar events. This gap-filling process tends to bias our interpretation of ambiguous events, and as such, a neutral or positive situation could be interpreted as negative. Stereotyped schematic information can colour new experiences, preventing the schema from evolving [23] and editing the event schema to account for this change. This acts as a type of schematic self-sabotage as individuals get stuck in a cycle of biased interpretations and ultimately inappropriate avoidance and reinforcing behaviour. Application of event schemas should, therefore, be monitored to prevent such a cycle from being started.
Role schemas
editRole schemas are defined as frameworks from which knowledge about a personal role in society influences the interpretation and production of behaviour [24]. Schemas organised in this category can be used to understand different role expectations between and within certain cultures [25]. Alternative cultures will have differing social norms and expectations when discussing the same societal roles. For example, more traditional cultural settings would hold the rigid stereotype schema that men are more likely to be managers in corporate settings [26].An individual, therefore, may assume that when interacting with women in this context, they would not be the manager. In comparison, more individualistic cultures may have a more flexible view and would not make this assumption, opting to wait to find out if this individual was, in fact, the manager.
As schemas are heavily influenced by previous experiences of social groups and categories within our own social and cultural norms, this could lead to misinterpretation when in alternative social contexts. Conflicts may arise as different social categories disagree on the role of an individual and, subsequently, the appropriate actions they should display in certain situations, causing tension or discrimination between groups[27] [28]. Alternatively, dual coding theory suggests the interpretation of information is better facilitated through the combination of verbal and non-verbal information. This explanation would provide a more comprehensive explanation of how cultural differences are bridged through nonverbal cues and universal gestures [29]. The assumed nature of social schema theory may hinder the transition of roles through cultures. However, this theory could still be present, with dual process theory supporting this culturally sensitive communication of role-related information.
Schema malfunction
editOne limitation of social schema application in everyday social situations is the possibility for them to malfunction and result in inappropriate behaviour. For example, an inappropriate schema may be selected for a specific event; a self-schema may focus on an internal characteristic and lead to the fixation of this trait, and forced rigidity of role schemas can lead to prejudice or bias. The increased likelihood of this occurring is influenced by an individual’s need to read a quick conclusion or judgment in a socially pressured environment [30].
Consistent interactions with similar events or people can lead to automatic assumptions, which can if reinforced, evolve into stereotypes. In these situations, someone may depend on stereotypical schemas and apply these in an event which does not fit the stereotypical assumptions, therefore causing them to act inappropriately. An example of this could include assuming that someone will hold the same political opinions and ideologies as you based on their physical appearance, situation or culture where you interacted with them. This could result in a potentially embarrassing environment where bias or conflict is created as a direct result of this assumption. Mismatching schema can also occur in a more theoretical context where written language may be misinterpreted to use one definition, where later on in the text, it is clear that the other definition was intended by the author [31]. Therefore, despite the large variety and application of schemas in different situations, their application may not be trustworthy, resulting in the need for caution and to check one’s intuition before automatically acting.
Future dirrections
editGaps in current research into schemas and, by extension, the four social subcategories indicate more investigation is required in the area of methodological development to quantify the production and use of schemas throughout social interactions. Questions have been raised as to how the process of developing a schema occurs and how limited schemas become more complex and specific over time. As such, further research could investigate the change in schema development based on environment or cognitive maturation.
References
edit- ^ Augoustinos, Martha; Innes, John Michael (September 1990). "Towards an integration of social representations and social schema theory". British Journal of Social Psychology. 29 (3): 213–231. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.1990.tb00901.x. ISSN 0144-6665.
- ^ Fiske, Susan T; Taylor, Shelley E (1991). Social cognition. New York: Mcgraw-Hill.
- ^ Kronenfeld, David (1988). "Review of Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, Volume 1: Theoretical Prerequisites; Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind". American Anthropologist. 90 (2): 431–433. doi:10.1525/aa.1988.90.2.02a00290. ISSN 0002-7294. JSTOR 677979.
- ^ Adekoya, Helen Odunola (2013). "Schema theory: A conceptual Review". Jornal of research and developement. 1 – via Babcock University, Ongun State, Nigeria.
- ^ Fiske, Susan T.; Kenny, David A.; Taylor, Shelley E. (1982-03-01). "Structural models for the mediation of salience effects on attribution". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 18 (2): 105–127. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(82)90046-4. ISSN 0022-1031.
- ^ Higgins, E. Tory (2022). Social Cognition: The Ontario Symposium Volume 1. Psychology Revivals Ser. C. Peter Herman, Mark P. Zanna. Milton: Taylor & Francis Group. ISBN 978-1-000-64910-9.
- ^ a b Mouchan, A; Bahmani, B; Askari, A (2016). "The Effectiveness of Schema Therapy on Reducing Symptoms of Emotional Breakdown". Health Sciences: 1–11.
- ^ Klerk, Noor de; Abma, Tineke A.; Bamelis, Lotte L. M.; Arntz, Arnoud (January 2017). "Schema Therapy for Personality Disorders: a Qualitative Study of Patients' and Therapists' Perspectives". Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy. 45 (1): 31–45. doi:10.1017/S1352465816000357. ISSN 1352-4658. PMID 27573409.
- ^ Ahadzadeh, Ashraf Sadat; Pahlevan Sharif, Saeed; Ong, Fon Sim (2017-03-01). "Self-schema and self-discrepancy mediate the influence of Instagram usage on body image satisfaction among youth". Computers in Human Behavior. 68: 8–16. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.11.011. ISSN 0747-5632.
- ^ Markus, Hazel Rose; Kitayama, Shinobu (1991). "Culture and the Self: Implications for Cognition, Emotion, and Motivation". Psychological Review. 98 (2): 224–253. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224.
- ^ Lemme, B.H (2006). Development of adutlhood. Boston, MA: Pearson Education, Inc.
- ^ Schunk, Dale H.; Zimmerman, Barry J. (2023-01-06). Self-regulation of Learning and Performance: Issues and Educational Applications. Taylor & Francis. ISBN 978-1-134-77721-1.
- ^ Leschziner, Vanina; Brett, Gordon (2021). "Have Schemas Been Good To Think With?". Sociological Forum. 36 (S1): 1207–1228. doi:10.1111/socf.12767. ISSN 1573-7861.
- ^ Newen, Albert; De Bruin, Leon; Gallagher, Shaun, eds. (2018). academic.oup.com. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.001.0001. ISBN 978-0-19-873541-0 https://academic.oup.com/edited-volume/28083. Retrieved 2024-11-29.
{{cite web}}
: Missing or empty|title=
(help) - ^ Herman, C. Peter; Zanna, Mark P.; Higgins, E. Tory (2022-11-01). Physical Appearance, Stigma, and Social Behavior: The Ontario Symposium Volume 3. Taylor & Francis. ISBN 978-1-000-64920-8.
- ^ Deutschmann, Mats; Steinvall, Anders (2020-01-01). "Combatting Linguistic Stereotyping and Prejudice by Evoking Stereotypes". Open Linguistics. 6 (1): 651–671. doi:10.1515/opli-2020-0036. ISSN 2300-9969.
- ^ Coltheart, Max; Davies, Martin (2022-01-02). "What is Capgras delusion?". Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 27 (1): 69–82. doi:10.1080/13546805.2021.2011185. ISSN 1354-6805. PMID 34890309.
- ^ Pandis, Charalampos; Agrawal, Niruj; Poole, Norman (2019-07-19). "Capgras' Delusion: A Systematic Review of 255 Published Cases". Psychopathology. 52 (3): 161–173. doi:10.1159/000500474. ISSN 0254-4962. PMID 31326968.
- ^ Melnychuk, Oksana (2023-07-26). "Schemas and Cognitive Modelling of Narrative". Věda a perspektivy. 7 (26). doi:10.52058/2695-1592-2023-7(26)-201-210. ISSN 2695-1592.
- ^ Baldassano, Christopher; Hasson, Uri; Norman, Kenneth A. (2018-11-07). "Representation of Real-World Event Schemas during Narrative Perception". The Journal of Neuroscience. 38 (45): 9689–9699. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0251-18.2018. ISSN 0270-6474. PMC 6222059. PMID 30249790.
- ^ Hard, Bridgette M.; Tversky, Barbara; Lang, David S. (2006-09-01). "Making sense of abstract events: Building event schemas". Memory & Cognition. 34 (6): 1221–1235. doi:10.3758/BF03193267. ISSN 1532-5946. PMID 17225504.
- ^ Dykema, J; Schaeffer, N. C. (2000). "Development of a Personal Event Schema". Center for Demography and Ecology, University of Madison-Wisconsin.
- ^ Gilligan, T. J.; Schwarz, G. (January 1976). "The self-association of adenosine-5'-triphosphate studied by circular dichroism at low ionic strengths". Biophysical Chemistry. 4 (1): 55–63. doi:10.1016/0301-4622(76)80007-5. ISSN 0301-4622. PMID 2349.
- ^ Augoustinos, Martha; Walker, Iain; Donaghue, Ngaire (2014-02-13). Social Cognition: An Integrated Introduction. SAGE. ISBN 978-1-4462-9725-4.
- ^ Tran, Ly Thi Phuong; Nguyen, Nhu Vo Tam (2023-09-30). "Similarities and Differences in Vietnamese-Japanese Language and Culture: A Study on the Domain of Words Indicating Kinship Relations from Role Schema Theory". Asia-Pacific Social Science Review. 23 (3). doi:10.59588/2350-8329.1504. ISSN 2350-8329.
- ^ Tabassum, Naznin; Nayak, Bhabani Shankar (2021-07-01). "Gender Stereotypes and Their Impact on Women's Career Progressions from a Managerial Perspective". IIM Kozhikode Society & Management Review. 10 (2): 192–208. doi:10.1177/2277975220975513. ISSN 2277-9752.
- ^ Gudykunst, William B. (2004). Bridging Differences: Effective Intergroup Communication. SAGE. ISBN 978-0-7619-2937-6.
- ^ Koester, Jolene; Lustig, Myron W. (2015-09-01). "Intercultural communication competence: Theory, measurement, and application". International Journal of Intercultural Relations. Intercultural Competence. 48: 20–21. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2015.03.006. ISSN 0147-1767.
- ^ "2024 10th International Conference on Applied System Innovation (ICASI) 17-21 April 2024 in Kyoto, Japan". 2024 10th International Conference on Applied System Innovation (ICASI). IEEE. 2024-04-17. pp. I–III. doi:10.1109/ICASI60819.2024.10547885. ISBN 979-8-3503-9492-4.
- ^ Pierro, Antonio; Kruglanski, Arie W. (2008-11-01). ""Seizing and Freezing" on a Significant-Person Schema: Need for Closure and the Transference Effect in Social Judgment". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 34 (11): 1492–1503. doi:10.1177/0146167208322865. ISSN 0146-1672. PMID 18948431.
- ^ Nair, P. Bhaskaran (2020-09-01). "Mismatches in the Contents of Reading and Writing Tasks: Revisiting Schema Theory". Journal of English Language Teaching. 62 (5): 27–31. ISSN 0973-5208.