Grand Valley Dani language

(Redirected from Upper Grand Valley Dani)

Grand Valley Dani, or simply Dani,[3] is one of the most populous Papuan languages in Indonesian New Guinea (also known as Papua). The Dani people live in the Baliem Valley of the Western Highlands.

Grand Valley Dani
Native toIndonesia
RegionHighland Papua
EthnicityDani and Lani[1]
Native speakers
(90,000 cited 1990–1996)[2]
Language codes
ISO 639-3Variously:
dni – Lower
dnt – Mid
dna – Upper
hap – Hupla
Glottologgran1246

Dialects

edit

Dialectical differentiation is great enough that Ethnologue assigns separate codes to three varieties:

  • Lower
  • Mid or Central, also known as Tulem
  • Upper

Lower Grand Valley Dani contains subdialects Lower Grand Valley Hitigima (Dani-Kurima, Kurima), Upper Bele, Lower Bele, Lower Kimbin (Kibin), and Upper Pyramid. Hupla, traditionally considered a separate language, is closer to Lower Grand Valley than the varieties of Grand Valley Dani are to each other.

Phonology

edit

Grand Valley Dani has established its own orthography during a conference between linguists of the Dutch New Guinea government and different missionary bodies in February 1961. This is the phonology of the Central Grand Valley Dani language:[4]

Consonants

edit
Consonants
Bilabial Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
plain lab.
Nasal m n ŋ
⟨ng⟩
Plosive voiceless p
⟨b, p⟩
t
⟨d, t⟩
k
⟨g, k⟩

⟨gw, kw⟩
ʔ
⟨'⟩
aspirated
⟨p, ph⟩

⟨t, th⟩

⟨k, kh⟩
kʷʰ
⟨kw, kwh⟩
implosive ɓ
⟨bp⟩
ɗ
⟨dl⟩
Fricative s h
Lateral l
Semivowel j w

Unlike other orthographies of local languages in Indonesia (largely based on the standard orthography), the original Grand Valley Dani orthography (the current one might be not known) has j instead of y, in common with the Indonesian old spelling.

  • The letters ⟨p, t, k⟩ are pronounced as aspirated /, , / in word-initial position and as [β, ɾ, ɣ] in intervocalic positions, respectively. They merge with voiceless /p, t, k/ syllable-finally, which is also represented by graphic voiced consonants ⟨b, d, g⟩. However, aspirated consonants still occur intervocalically.
    • Medial ⟨pp, tt, kk⟩ are either pronounced as /p, t, k/ or as geminated /, , /.[5]
  • The phoneme /h/ merges with preceding or following phonemes:[6]
    • It aspirates preceding ⟨p, t, k⟩, creating effectively phonemic aspirated consonants in intervocalic positions (japha /japʰa/ "they fought").
    • It also compensatorily lengthened adjacent vowel or sonorants (except /j/, /w/), however, one element of the most adjacent lengthened vowel to /h/ is devoiced (wamhe /wamhe/, [wamm̥ɛ] "pig (with connective morpheme)").

Vowels

edit
Vowels
front central back
close i u
ɪ
⟨y⟩
ʊ
⟨v⟩
mid e o
low a

Grammar

edit

Verbs

edit

Verbs in Grand Valley Dani are highly inflected for many tenses. Infinitive is marked by the suffix -in, although verb stems in -s- change to -t- before consonants: wetasin "to roast", but wetathy "I roasted".

Finite tenses

edit
Default personal markers[7]
number
Singular Plural
person
1st -y -o
2nd -en -ep
3rd -e -em

Although there are claimed "default" personal markers, the correspondences between tense suffixes and personal markers are often highly irregular. Nevertheless, inflections of verbs are still highly regular. Unless denoted in the table, verb forms are marked by personal markers.

List
Tense Suffix(es)
Near future -ikin in the singular, -ukun in the plural.
Never inflected by person, only by number.
Indefinite future -isikin in the singular, -isukun in the plural.
Never inflected by person, only by number.
Near past -h-.
Remote past -hikh- in the 3SG, -hukh- in the 3PL, and -hVk- elsewhere.
-V- is an echo vowel from the personal markers, e.g. -hyky, -heken, etc.
Perfect past Suffixing the near past with -tik in the 1SG, -ttik in the 2SG, -sip in the 2PL and -sik elsewhere.
In the second person, the final consonants of original near past endings, when suffixed, have to be deleted (-hen (2SG) + -ttik-hettik, -hep (2PL) + -sip-hesip).
The ending for 3PL is irregular: -hasik instead of *-hemsik.
Habitual Replacing every instances of syllable-final -i- and -sik (but not -sip-sep) of the perfect past with -e- and -tek, respectively (-hettik-hettek).
Habitual perfect Infixing -si- into the main habitual ending (-hettek-hettesik).
The ending for 2PL is irregular: -hesep-hetesip instead of *-hesesip.
Singular Plural
First Second Third First Second Third
Future Near -ikin -ukun
Indefinite -isikin -isukun
Past Near -hy -hen -he -ho -hep -hem
Remote -hyky -heken -hikhe -huku -hikip -hukha
Perfect -hytik -hettik -hesik -hosik -hesip -hasik
Habitual Main -hytek -hettek -hetek -hotek -hesep -hatek
Perfect -hytesik -hettesik -hetesik -hotesik -hetesip -hatesik
Progressive -hylahy -hylaken -iako -hylako -hylakep -iakoei

Semantics

edit

The Dani language differentiates only two basic colours, mili for cool/dark shades such as blue, green, and black, and mola for warm/light colours such as red, yellow, and white. This trait makes it an interesting field of research for language psychologists, such as Eleanor Rosch, investigating the Whorf hypothesis.[8][9]

References

edit
  1. ^ Tabuni, Gasper (2017). Kunume Wene-Nya Masyarakat Adat Balim: Studi Kasus Makna 'Kunume Wene' dalam Perilaku Orang Kombarabuni dalam Jangkauan Zaman (Thesis). Magister Studi Pembangunan Program Pascasarjana UKSW. Retrieved 2024-06-18.
  2. ^ Lower at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
    Mid at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
    Upper at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
    Hupla at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
  3. ^ Compare Lani
  4. ^ van der Stap 1966, p. 10–11
  5. ^ Bromley 1961, p. 35
  6. ^ Bromley 1961, p. 31–32
  7. ^ van der Stap 1966, p. 22–23
  8. ^ Saunders, Barbara Ann Christine (1992). The Invention of Basic Colour Terms. R.U.U.-I.S.O.R. ISBN 9789051870879 – via Google Books.
  9. ^ Heider, Eleanor Rosch (1972). "Probabilities, Sampling, and Ethnographic Method: The Case of Dani Colour Names". Man. 7 (3): 448–466. doi:10.2307/2800917. JSTOR 2800917.

Further reading

edit