Devon--nice job. I made a few minor edits and added a relevant citation. Feel free to post it to the live page. --ShaneTMueller (talk) 22:20, 27 February 2012 (UTC)

Screenshot of the PEBL psychology software running the Tower of London test

The Tower of London test is a well-known test used in applied clinical neuropsychology for the assessment of executive functioning specifically to detect deficits in planning,[1] which may occur due to a variety of medical and neuropsychiatric conditions. It is related to the classic problem-solving puzzle known as the Tower of Hanoi.

The test consists of two boards with pegs and several beads with different colors. The examiner (usually a clinical psychologist or a neuropsychologist) uses the beads and the boards to present the examinee with problem-solving tasks. Several variants of the test exist. Shallice's original test used three beads and pegs with different heights, although later researchers have generalized this to more beads without a peg height restriction.[2] Versions of the test are available from a number of sources, including a stand-alone test by William Culbertson and Eric Zillmer (published by Drexel University) and a child/adolescent version that is part of the original NEPSY neuropsychological battery of tests by Marit Korkman, Ursula Kirk, and Sally Kemp (although removed from the second edition). A computerized variant, known as the Stockings of Cambridge test, is available as part of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB). This test is also available within the Psychology Experiment Building Language Battery (PEBL) [3].

One common use is for diagnosis of executive impairment. Subjects that require more moves and take more time to achieve completion are suspects to decreased cognitive performance in regards to planning. The performance of the examinee is compared to representative samples of individuals the same age in order to derive hypotheses about the person's executive cognitive ability, especially as it may relate to brain damage. A certain degree of controversy surrounds the test's construct validity.[4]

References

edit
  1. ^ Shallice, T. (1982). "Specific impairments of planning". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 298 (1089): 199–209. doi:10.1098/rstb.1982.0082. PMID 6125971.
  2. ^ Phillips, L.H.; Wynn, V.; Gilhooly, K. J.; Della Sala, S.; Logie, R. H. (1999). "The role of memory in the Tower of London task". Memory. 7 (2): 209–231. doi:10.1080/741944066. PMID 10645380.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  3. ^ Piper, Brian J.; Li, Victoria; Eiwaz, Massarra A.; Kobel, Yuliyana V.; Benice, Ted S.; Chu, Alex M.; Olsen, Reid H. J.; Rice, Douglas Z.; Gray, Hilary M.; Mueller, Shane T.; Raber, J. (2011-05-02). "Executive function on the Psychology Experiment Building Language tests". Behavior Research Methods. 44 (1): 110–123. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0096-6. ISSN 1554-3528. PMC 3705215. PMID 21534005.
  4. ^ KAFER, K. L. (1997). "On Testing the Face Validity of Planning/Problem-Solving Tasks in a Normal Population". Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 3 (2): 108–119. doi:10.1017/S1355617797001082. Retrieved 2010-03-04. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
edit


Category:Neuropsychological tests Category:Cognitive tests