History

edit

Current research literature documents well that human beings are poor detectors of deception.[1] The research reveals that while people show a reliable ability to tell truth from deception, accuracy rates are only a little above chance (54%).[2]

Interpersonal Deception Theory (IDT) attempts to explain the manner in which individuals engaged in face-to-face communication deal with actual or perceived deception on the conscious and subconscious levels. IDT proposes that the majority of individuals overestimate their ability to detect deception. In some cultures, various means of deception are acceptable while other forms are not. Acceptance of deception can be found in language terms that classify, rationalize or condemn, such behavior. Deception that may be considered a simple white lie to save feelings may me be determined socially acceptable, while deception used to gain certain advantages can be determined to be ethically questionable. It has been estimated that “deception and suspected deception arise in at least one quarter of all conversations”.[3]

Interpersonal deception detection between partners is difficult unless a partner tells an outright lie or contradicts something the other partner knows is true. While it is difficult to deceive a person over a long period of time, deception often occurs in day-to-day conversations between relational partners.[4] Detecting deception is difficult because it places a significant cognitive load on the deceiver. The deceiver must recall previous statements so that their story remains consistent and believable. As a result, deceivers often leak important information both verbally and nonverbally.

 
Sigmund Freud (1856-1939)

In the early nineteenth century, Sigmund Freud studied nonverbal cues to detect deception. Freud observed a patient being asked about his darkest feelings. If his mouth was shut and his fingers were trembling, Freud considered the subject to be lying. Freud also noted other nonverbal cues, such as drumming one's fingers when telling a lie. More recently, scientists have attempted to establish the differences between truthful and deceptive behavior using a myriad of psychological and physiological approaches. In 1969, Ekman and Friesen used straightforward observation methods to determine deceptive non-verbal leakage cues,[5] while more recently Rosenfeldet et al. used magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) to detect differences between honest and deceptive responses.[6]

In 1989, DePaulo and Kirkendol developed the Motivation Impairment Effect (MIE). MIE states the harder people try to deceive others, the more likely they are to get caught. Burgoon and Floyd, however, revisited this research and formed the idea that deceivers are more active in their attempt to deceive than most would anticipate or expect.

IDT was developed in 1996 by David B. Buller and Judee K. Burgoon.[7] Prior to their study, deception had not been fully considered as a communication activity. Previous work had focused upon the formulation of principles of deception. These principles were derived by evaluating the lie detection ability of individuals observing unidirectional communication. These early studies found initially that "although humans are far from infallible in their efforts to diagnose lies, they are substantially better at the task than would result merely by chance."[8] Buller and Burgoon discount the value of highly controlled studies – usually one-way communication experiments – designed to isolate unmistakable cues that people are lying. Therefore, IDT is based on two-way communication and intended to describe deception as an interaction communicative process.[9]

Buller and Burgoon initially based their theory of IPD on the four-factor model of deception developed by social psychologist Miron Zuckerman, who argues that the four components of deceit inevitably cause cognitive overload and therefore leakage. Zuckerman's four factors include the attempt to control information, which fosters behavior that can come across as too practiced, followed by physiological arousal as a result of deception. This arousal then leads to the third factor, felt emotions, which are usually guilt and anxiety, which can become noticeable to an observer. Additionally, the many cognitive factors and mental gymnastics that are going on during a deception often lead to nonverbal leakage cues, such as increased blinking and a higher pitched voice.


Online dating

edit

Research on the use of deception in online dating has shown that people are generally truthful about themselves with the exception of physical attributes to appear more attractive.[10] Most online deception is subtle with slight exaggerations, representing people’s attempts to portray themselves in the best possible light.[10] Of all online contexts, online dating appears the most prone to deception. In general, no matter the setting, people are more likely to be deceptive when looking for a date than in other social situations.[11]

Research suggests that while slight misrepresentations on online dating sites are quite common, major deceptions are actually rare. It seems that those who engage in online dating realize that while they want to make the best possible impression, if they want to pursue an offline relationship, they can’t begin it with outright falsehoods that will quickly be revealed.[12] One survey of over 5,000 users of online dating sites how likely they were to misrepresent themselves in areas such as appearance and job information.[13] The average rating on these items was a 2 on a 10-point scale, indicating a relatively low level of deception overall.

Some people are more prone to deceptive behavior online than others, such as those with high sensation-seeking tendencies, and those who show addictive behavior toward the Internet.[14] Conversely, those who are introverted or have high tendencies for social anxiety are especially likely to be honest about their personalities online, revealing hidden aspects of the self that they would not normally show to others offline. [15] [16]

According to the Scientific American, “nine out of ten online daters will fib about their height, weight, or age” such that men were more likely to lie about height while women were more likely to lie about weight.[17] In addition, those high in the trait of self-monitoring are more likely to be dishonest on dating websites. In all aspects of their social lives, self-monitors are concerned with outward appearance and adapt their behavior to match the social situation. Thus, they also tend to be more deceptive in their attempts to attract dates both offline[11] and online.[13]

In a study conducted by Toma and Hancock, “less attractive people were found to be more likely to have chosen a profile picture in which they were significantly more attractive than they were in everyday life."[18] Both genders used this strategy in online dating profiles, but women more so than men.[18] Additionally, the researchers found that those deemed less attractive were more likely to express deception in the areas of physical attractiveness such as height and weight.

A qualitative study investigated deception in online dating. The study focused on four questions: (1) About what characteristics are online daters deceptive? (2) What motivation do online daters have for their deception of others in the online-dating environment? (3) What perceptions do online daters have about other daters' deceit towards them in the online-dating environment? (4) How does deception affect romantic relationships formed in the online-dating environment? In an online survey, data was collected from 15 open-ended questions. The study had 52 participants, ranging in age from 21 to 37, and found that most online daters consider themselves (and others) mostly honest in their online self-presentation. Online daters who used deception were motivated to do so by the desire to attract partners and project a positive self-image. Daters were willing to overlook deception in others if they viewed the dishonesty as a slight exaggeration or a characteristic of little value to the dater. Despite deception, participants believe that the online-dating environment can develop successful romantic relationships.

Criticism

edit

DePaulo, Ansfield and Bell questioned IDT: "We can find the 'why' question in Buller and Burgoon's synthesis. There is no intriguing riddle or puzzle that needs to be solved, and no central explanatory mechanism is ever described." Although they praised Buller and Burgoon's 18 propositions as a comprehensive description of the timeline of deceptive interactions, they said the propositions lacked the interconnectedness and predictive power of a unifying theory. DePaulo et al. criticized IDT for failing to distinguish between interactive communication (which emphasizes the situational and contextual aspects of communicative exchanges) from interpersonal communication, which emphasizes exchanges in which the sender and receiver make psychological predictions about the other's behavior based on specific prior knowledge; this conceptual ambiguity limited IDT's explanatory power.

Park and Levine provide additional commentary questioning IDT stating that “because both interactive and noninteractive experiments lead to the same conclusions about truth-bias and accuracy regardless of interactivity, interactivity is not the all-important consideration as IDT claims.”[19] In IDT, a crucial emphasis is placed in the aspect of interactivity to determine deception detection accuracy. However, Park and Levine do not see an empirical basis for this foundational claim of IDT.


  1. ^ Burgoon, Judee K.; Blair, J. Pete; Strom, Renee E. (2008). "Cognitive Biases and Nonverbal Cue Availability in Detecting Deception". Human Communication Research. 34 (4): 572–599. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2008.00333.x. ISSN 1468-2958.
  2. ^ Bond, Charles F.; DePaulo, Bella M. (2006). "Accuracy of Deception Judgments". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 10 (3): 214–234. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_2. ISSN 1088-8683.
  3. ^ "PsycNET". psycnet.apa.org. Retrieved 2019-09-29.
  4. ^ Laura, Guerrero (2007). Close Encounters: Communication in Relationships (2nd ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. ISBN 150637672X.
  5. ^ Ekman, Paul; Friesen, Wallace V. (1969-02-01). "Nonverbal Leakage and Clues to Deception". Psychiatry. 32 (1): 88–106. doi:10.1080/00332747.1969.11023575. ISSN 0033-2747. PMID 27785970.
  6. ^ Ganis, Giorgio; Rosenfeld, J. Peter; Meixner, John; Kievit, Rogier A.; Schendan, Haline E. (2011-03-01). "Lying in the scanner: Covert countermeasures disrupt deception detection by functional magnetic resonance imaging". NeuroImage. 55 (1): 312–319. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.025. ISSN 1053-8119.
  7. ^ Buller D.B & Burgoon, J.K (1996). Interpersonal Deception Theory. Communication Theory (6) 3. 202-242.
  8. ^ DePaulo, B.M., Zukerman, M., & Rosenthal, R. (1980, Spring). Humans As Lie Detectors. Journal of Communication, 30 (2) 129-139.
  9. ^ https://managingresearchlibrary.org/glossary/communicative-interaction
  10. ^ a b Zimbler, Mattitiyahu; Feldman, Robert S. (2011). "Liar, Liar, Hard Drive on Fire: How Media Context Affects Lying Behavior". Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 41 (10): 2492–2507. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2011.00827.x. ISSN 1559-1816.
  11. ^ a b Rowatt, Wade C.; Cunningham, Michael R.; Druen, Perri B. (1999-04-01). "Lying to Get a Date: The Effect of Facial Physical Attractiveness on the Willingness to Deceive Prospective Dating Partners". Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 16 (2): 209–223. doi:10.1177/0265407599162005. ISSN 0265-4075.
  12. ^ Toma, Catalina L.; Hancock, Jeffrey T.; Ellison, Nicole B. (2008-08-01). "Separating Fact From Fiction: An Examination of Deceptive Self-Presentation in Online Dating Profiles". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 34 (8): 1023–1036. doi:10.1177/0146167208318067. ISSN 0146-1672.
  13. ^ a b Hall, Jeffrey A.; Park, Namkee; Song, Hayeon; Cody, Michael J. (2010-02-01). "Strategic misrepresentation in online dating: The effects of gender, self-monitoring, and personality traits". Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 27 (1): 117–135. doi:10.1177/0265407509349633. ISSN 0265-4075.
  14. ^ Lu, Hung-Yi (2008-04-01). "Sensation-Seeking, Internet Dependency, and Online Interpersonal Deception". CyberPsychology & Behavior. 11 (2): 227–231. doi:10.1089/cpb.2007.0053. ISSN 1094-9313.
  15. ^ Amichai-Hamburger, Yair; Wainapel, Galit; Fox, Shaul (2002-4). ""On the Internet no one knows I'm an introvert": extroversion, neuroticism, and Internet interaction". Cyberpsychology & Behavior: The Impact of the Internet, Multimedia and Virtual Reality on Behavior and Society. 5 (2): 125–128. doi:10.1089/109493102753770507. ISSN 1094-9313. PMID 12025878. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  16. ^ McKenna, Katelyn Y. A.; Green, Amie S.; Gleason, Marci E. J. (2002). "Relationship Formation on the Internet: What's the Big Attraction?". Journal of Social Issues. 58 (1): 9–31. doi:10.1111/1540-4560.00246. ISSN 1540-4560.
  17. ^ "The Truth about Online Dating". Scientific American. Retrieved 2019-09-29.
  18. ^ a b Hancock, Jeffrey; Toma, Catalina; Ellison, Nicole (2007-01-01). "The truth about lying in online dating profiles": 449–452. doi:10.1145/1240624.1240697. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  19. ^ Park, Hee Sun; Levine, Timothy R. (2015). "Base Rates, Deception Detection, and Deception Theory: A Reply to Burgoon (2015)". Human Communication Research. 41 (3): 350–366. doi:10.1111/hcre.12066. ISSN 1468-2958.