Wikipedia:School and university projects/Psyc3330 w10/Group17
Procedural memory is our memory for how to do things. When needed, procedural memories are automatically retrieved and utilized for the execution of the step-by-step procedures involved in both cognitive and motor skills; from tying your shoes to flying an airplane. This process occurs without the need for conscious control or attention. Procedural memory is a type of long-term memory and more specifically a type of implicit memory.
History
editThe difference between procedural and declarative memory systems was first explored and understood with simple semantics. Psychologists and Philosophers began writing about memory over a century ago. "Mechanical memory" was first noted in 1804 by Maine de Biran. William James, within his famous book: Principles of Psychology(1890), suggested that there was a difference between memory and habit. Cognitive psychology disregarded the influence of learning on memory systems in its early years, and this greatly limited the research conducted in procedural learning up until the 20th century[1]. With the turn of the century brought a clearer understanding of the functions and structures involved in procedural memory acquisition, storage and retrieval processes. McDougall (1923) first made the distinction between explicit and implicit memory. In the 1970s procedural and declarative knowledge was distinguished in literature on artificial intelligence. Studies in the 1970’s, divided and moved towards two areas of work, one focusing on animal studies and the other to amnesic patients. The first convincing experimental evidence for a dissociation between declarative (“knowing what”) and non-declarative or procedural (“knowing how”) memory was from Milner (1962), by demonstrating that a severely amnesic patient, H.M., could learn a hand–eye coordination skill (mirror drawing) in the absence of any memory of having practiced the task before. Although this finding indicated that memory was not made up of a single system positioned in one place in the brain, at the time, others agreed that motor skills are likely a special case that represented a less cognitive form of memory. However, by refining and improving experimental measures, there has been extensive research using amnesic patients varying locations and degrees of structural damage. Increased work with amnesic patients lead to the finding that they were able to retain and learn tasks other than motor skills. However, these findings had shortcomings in how they were perceived as amnesic patients sometimes fell short on normal levels of performance and therefore amnesia was viewed as strictly a retrieval deficit. Further studies with amnesic patients found a larger domain of normally functioning memory for skill abilities. For example, using a mirror reading task, amnesic patients showed performance at a normal rate, even though they are unable to remember some of the words that they were reading. In the 1980’s much was discovered about the anatomy physiology of the mechanisms involved in procedural memory. The cerebellum, hippocampus, neostriatum, and basal ganglia were identified as being involved in memory acquisition tasks.[2].
Acquisition of skill
editThe acquisition of skill requires practice. Merely repeating a task alone however, does not ensure the acquisition of a skill. Skill acquisition is than achieved when an observed behaviour has changed due to experience or practice. This is known as learning and is not directly observable [3]. The information processing model, which incorporates this idea of experience, proposes that skills develop from the interaction of four components central to information processing [3]. These components include: processing speed; rate at which information is processed in our processing system, breadth of declarative knowledge; size of an individual's factual information store, breadth of procedural skill; ability to perform the actual skill and processing capacity; synonymous with working memory. The processing capacity is of importance to procedural memory because through the process of proceduralization an individual stores procedural memory. This improves skill usage by linking environmental cues with appropriate responses.
An important model for understanding skill acquisition was proposed by Fitts (1954) and his colleagues. This model proposed the idea that learning was possible through the completion of various stages. The stages involved, include:
- Cognitive phase[4][5]
- Associative phase[4][5]
- Autonomous phase (also called the procedural phase)[4][5]
Cognitive phase
editAt this point in Fitts' (1954) model of skill acquisition individuals come to understand what an observed skill is composed of. Attention at this point in the process is significant for the acquisition of skill. This process involves breaking down the desired skill to be learned into parts and understanding how these parts come together as a whole for them correct performance of the task. The way an individual organizes these parts is known as schemas. Schemas are important in directing the acquisition process and the way we come to choose schemas is described by metacognition. To learn more, see metacognition. [4][5]
Associative phase
editThe associative phase of the Fitts (1954) model involves individuals repeat practice until patterns or responding emerge. At this part in the model, actions of the skill become learned (or automated) as ineffective actions are dropped. An individual's sensory system acquires the accurate spatial and symbolic data required for the completion of the skill. The ability to differentiate important from unimportant stimuli is crucial at this stage of the model. It is held that the greater the amount of important stimuli associated with a task, the longer it will take to complete this phase of the model.[4][5]
Autonomous phase
editThis is the final phase in Fitts (1954) model, and it involves perfecting skill acquisition. The ability to discriminate important from unimportant stimuli are made quicker and less thought process is required because the skill has become automated. Important to this phase of the model is experience and factual knowledge store for the observed skill.[4][5]
Expertise
editDivided attention
editThere are several factors that contribute to the exceptional performance of a skill: memory capacities[6] [7], knowledge structures [8], problem-solving abilities [9], and attentional abilities [10]. They all play key roles, each with its own degree of importance based on the procedures and skills required, the context, and the intended goals of the performance. Using these individualized abilities to compare how experts and novices differ regarding both cognitive and sensorimotor skills has provided a wealth of insight into what makes an expert excellent, and conversely, what sorts of mechanisms do novice lack. Evidence suggests that an often overlooked condition for skill excellence is attentional mechanisms involved in the effective utilization and deployment of procedural memory during the real-time execution of skills. Research suggests that early in skill learning, execution is controlled by a set of unintegrated procedural steps that are held in working memory and attended to one-by-one in a step-by-step fashion[11] [12] [13]. The problem with this is that attention is a limited resource. Therefore this step-by-step process of controlling task performance occupies attentional capacity which in turn reduces the performer’s ability to focus on other aspects of the performance, such as decision making, fine motor-skills, self-monitoring of energy level and “seeing the field or ice or court”. However, with practice, procedural knowledge develops, which operates largely outside of working memory, and thus allows for skills to be executed more automatically[14] [15]. This, of course, has a very positive effect on overall performance by freeing the mind of the need to closely monitor and attend to the more basic, mechanical skills, so that attention can be paid to other processes [10] .
Choking under pressure
editIt is well established that highly practiced, over-learned skills are performed automatically; they are controlled in real time, supported by procedural memory, require little attention, and operate largely outside of working memory[16]. However, sometimes even9 experienced and highly skilled performers falter under conditions of stress. This phenomenon is commonly referred to as choking, and serves as a very interesting exception to the general rule that well-learned skills are robust and resistant to deterioration across a wide range of conditions[17]. Although not well understood, it is widely accepted that the underlying cause of choking is performance pressure, which has been defined as an anxious desire to perform very well in a given situation[17]. Choking is most often associated with motor skills, and the most common real-life instances are in sports. We have all seen highly trained professional athletes deteriorate under pressure and perform so poorly, that in the moment, they seem like beginners. However, choking can occur within any domain that demands a high level of performance involving complex cognitive, verbal or motor skills. “Self-focus” theories suggest that pressure increases anxiety and self-consciousness about performing correctly, which in turn causes an increase in attention paid to the processes directly involved in the execution of the skill[17]. This attention to the step-by-step procedure disrupts the well-learned, automatic (proceduralized) performance. What was once an effortless and unconscious retrieval execution of a procedural memory becomes slow and deliberate[18][19][20][21]. Evidence suggests that the more automated a skill is the more resistant it is to distractions, performance pressure and subsequent choking. This serves as a good example of the relative durability of procedural memory over episodic memory. In addition to deliberate practice and automization of skills, self-consciousness training has been shown to help with reducing the effect of choking under pressure[17].
Famous examples of choking
edit- 1996 Masters golf tournament, Greg Norman lost to Nick Faldo
- 1993 Wimbledon women's final, Jana Novotna lost to Steffi Graf
Expertise-induced amnesia
editThis phenomenon is based on the assumption that reducing or diverting the amount of attention paid to material being encoded and stored will reduce the quality and quantity of the later retrieval of that material in a form that is explicit and reportable. So, if a well learned skill is stored as a procedural memory, and its retrieval and subsequent performance is mostly unconscious and automatic, there is evidence showing that the explicit recollection of what happened during the performance will be reduced[17]. A recent example illustrates this concept nicely. Immediately following Sidney Crosby’s overtime goal against the USA, winning the 2010 Olympic Gold Medal for Canada in men’s hockey, a reporter with TSN did an on-ice interview with Crosby: “ Sid, if you can, just take us through how that goal went in” Crosby replied: “I don’t really remember, I just shot it – I think from around here. That’s all I really remember. I think it went 5-hole, but, um, I didn’t really see it to be honest" [22]. A reported 16.6 million Canadians watched Sidney's goal, and it's safe to assume that most of them would have been able to easily recollect the details, but not Crosby, he was immersed in an automatic state, that at least temporarily, blocked his ability to remember.
Anatomical structures
editStriatum and basal ganglia
editThe dorsolateral striatum is associated with the acquisition of habits and is the main neuronal cell nucleus linked to procedural memory. Connecting excitatory afferent nerve fibers help in the regulation of activity in the basal ganglia circuit. Essentially, two parallel information processing pathways diverge from the striatum, both acting in opposition to each other in the control of movement, they allow for association with other needed functional structures [23] One pathway is direct while the other is indirect and all pathways work together to allow for a functional neural feedback loop. Many looping circuits connect back at the striatum from other areas of the brain; including those from the emotion-center linked limbic cortex, the reward-center linked ventral striatum and other important motor regions related to movement. [24] The main looping circuit involved in the motor skill part of procedural memory is usually called the cortex-basal ganglia-thalamus-cortex loop. [25]
The striatum is unique because it doesn't have the ever-so popular glutamate-related neurons found throughout most of the brain. Instead, it is categorized by a high concentration of a special type of GABA related inhibiting cell known as the medium spiny neuron. [26]. The two parallel pathways previously mentioned, travel to and from the striatum and are made up of these same special medium spiny neurons. These neurons are all sensitive to different neurotransmitters and contain a variety of corresponding receptors including dopamine receptors (DRD1, DRD2),Muscarinic receptors (M4) and Adenosine receptors (A2A). Separate interneurons are known to communicate with striatal spiny neurons in the presence of the somatic nervous system neurotransmitter acetylcholine. [27]
Current understanding of brain anatomy and physiology suggests that striatal neural plasticity is what allows basal ganglia circuits to communicate between structures and to functionally operate in procedural memory processing. [28]
Cerebellum
editThe Cerebellum is famous for its role in correcting movement and for its ability to fine tune the motor agility found in procedural skills such as painting, instrument playing and even in golf. Damage to this are may prevent the proper relearning of motor skills and through associated research it has more recently been linked to having a role in automating the unconscious process used when learning a procedural skill. [29] New thoughts in the scientific community suggest that the cerebellar cortex holds the holy grail of memory, what is known to researchers as "the engram" or the biological place where memory lives. The initial memory trace is thought to form here and then travel outwards to other brain nuclei for consolidation via parallel fibers known as Purkinje cells. [30]
Limbic system
editThe limbic system is a group of unique brain areas that work together in many interrelated processes involved in emotion, motivation, learning and memory. Current thinking indicates that the limbic system shares anatomy with a component of the neostriatum already credited with the major task of controlling procedural memory. Once thought to be functionally separate, this vital section of the brain found on the striatum’s back border has only recently been linked to memory and is now being called the marginal division zone (MrD). [31] A special membrane protein associated with the limbic system is said to concentrate in related structures and to travel towards the basal nuclei. To put things simply, the activation of brain regions that work together during procedural memory can be followed because of this limbic system associated membrane protein and its application in molecular and immunohistochemistry research. [32]
Physiology involved
editDopamine
editDopamine is one of the more known neuromodulators involved in procedural memory. Evidence suggests that it may influence neural plasticity in memory systems by adapting brain processing when the environment is changing and an individual is then forced to make a behavioural choice or series of rapid decisions. It is very important in the process of “adaptive navigation,” which serves to help different brain areas respond together during a new situation that has many unknown stimuli and features. [33] Dopamine pathways are dispersed all over the brain and this allows for parallel processing in many structures all at the same time. Currently most research points to the mesocorticolimbic dopamine pathway as the system most related to reward learning and psychological conditioning. [34]
At the synapse
editRecent findings could help explain the relationship between procedural memory, learning and synaptic plasticity at the level of the molecule. One study used small animals lacking normal levels of CREB family transcription factors to look at the processing of information in the striatum during various learning tasks. Although poorly understood, results show that CREB function is needed at the synapse for linking the acquisition and storage of procedural memory. [35]
Tests
editPursuit rotor task
editA device used to study motor learning by requiring the participant to follow a moving object with a cursor or use a stylus to follow the target on a computer screen or a turntable[36]. This displays a measurement of procedural memory as well as demonstrates the participant's fine-motor skills[37]. The results are then calculated by the participants time-on and time-off the object. Amnesic participants show no impairment in this motor task when tested at later trials. It does however, seem to be affected by the lack of sleep and drug use one is exposed to[38].
Serial reaction time task
editThis task involves having participants retain and learn procedural skills that assess specific memory for procedural-motor skill[39]. These skills are measured by observing the speed and accuracy of the participant's ability to retain and acquire new skills. The reaction time is the time it takes for the participant to respond to the designated cue presented to them.[40]. Participants with Alzheimer's disease and amnesia demonstrate a long retention time which indicates that they are able to retain the skill and demonstrate effective performance of the task at a later point in time. [40]
Mirror tracing task
editThis task is more specifically looks at the integration of the senses as it is a visual motor test where the participants learn a new motor skill involving hand-eye coordination[37]. Evidence is shown for procedural memory as amnesic participants are able to learn and retain this task. Drawing the image is the work of your procedural memory; once you figure out how to draw the image in the mirror you have little difficulty the second time. Individuals with Alzheimer’s disease are not able recall the skills acquired in a mirror tracing task, but they acquire the procedural performance ability regardless.[40]
Weather prediction task
editSpecifically, this task uses experimental analysis of weather prediction. As a probability learning task, the participant is required to indicate what strategy they are using to solve the task. It is a cognitively-oriented task that is learned in a procedural manner[40]. It's designed using multidimensional stimuli, so participants are given a set of cards with shapes and then asked to predict the outcome. After the prediction is made participants receive feedback and make a classification based on that feedback[41]. For example, the participant can be shown one pattern and then asked to predict whether the pattern indicates good or bad weather. The actual weather outcome will be determined by a probabilistic rule based off of each individual card. Amnesic participants learn this task in training but are impaired in later training control[41].
Disorders
editDisorders have been important for the understanding of memory systems. The memory abilities and inhibitions of patients suffering from various diseases played a major role in establishing the distinction that long term memory consists of different types of memory, more specifically declarative memory and procedural memory. Furthermore, they have been important for illuminating the structures of the brain that comprise the neural network of procedural memory.
Alzheimer's disease and dementia
editCurrent Research indicates that procedural memory problems in Alzheimer’s may be caused by changes in enzyme activity in memory-integrating brain regions such as the hippocampus. The specific enzyme linked to these changes is called acetylcholinesterase (AchE) which may be affected by a genetic predisposition in an immune-system brain receptor called the histamine H1 receptor. The same current scientific information also looks at how dopamine, serotonin and acetylcholine neurotransmitter levels vary in the cerebellum of patients that have this disease. Modern findings advance the idea that the histamine system may be responsible for the cognitive deficits found in Alzheimer's and for the potential procedural memory problems that may develop as a result of the psychopathology. [42]
Gilles de la tourette syndrome
editThis central nervous system syndrome like many other procedural-memory related disorders involves changes in the associated subcortical brain area known as the striatum. This area and the brain circuits closely interacting with it from the basal ganglia are affected both structurally and at a more functional level in the people affected by Gilles De La Tourette Syndrome; more commonly referred to as Tourette Syndrome. Current literature on this topic provides evidence for there being many unique forms of procedural memory. Interestingly, the one most relevant to procedural memory and most common in Tourette's is related to the skill-acquisition process that ties stimuli to response during the learning part of procedural memory. [43]
Human immunodeficiency virus (HIV)
editNeural systems used by procedural memory are commonly targeted by Human Immunodeficiency Virus; the striatum being the structure most notably affected. [44] MRI studies have even shown white matter irregularity and basal ganglia subcortical atrophy in these vital areas necessary for both procedural memory and motor-skill. [45] Applied research using various procedural memory tasks such as the Rotary pursuit, Mirror star tracing and Weather prediction tasks have shown that HIV positive individuals perform worse than HIV negative participants suggesting that poorer overall performance on tasks is due to the specific changes in the brain caused by the disease. [46]
Huntington's disease
editDespite being a disorder that directly affects striatal areas of the brain used in procedural memory, most individual’s with huntington’s disease don’t display the same memory problems as other people with striatum related brain diseases. [47] In more advanced stages of the disease, however, procedural memory is affected by damage to the important brain pathways that help the inner subcortical and prefrontal cortex parts of the brain to communicate. [48]
Obsessive compulsive disorder
editNeuroimaging studies show that OCD patients perform considerably better on procedural memory tasks because of noticeable over-activation of the striatum brain structures, specifically the frontostriatal circuit. These studies suggest that procedural memory in OCD patients is unusually improved in the early learning stages of procedural memory. [49]
Parkinson's disease
editParkinson’s Disease is known to affect selective areas in the frontal lobe area of the brain. Current scientific information suggests that the memory performance problems notably shown in patients are controlled by unusual frontostriatal circuits. [50] Parkinson’s patients often have difficulty with the sequence-specific knowledge that is needed in the acquisition step of procedural memory. [51] Further evidence suggests that the frontal lobe networks relate to executive function and only act when specific tasks are presented to the patient. This tells us that the frontostriatal circuits are independent but able to work collaboratively with other areas of the brain to help with various things such as paying attention or focusing. [52]
Schizophrenia
editMRI studies have shown that schizophrenic patients not currently taking related medication have a smaller putamen; part of the striatum that plays a very important role in procedural memory. [53] Further studies on the brain reveal that schizophrenics]have improper basal ganglia communication with the surrounding extrapyramidal system that is known to be closely involved with the motor system and in the coordination of movement. [54] The most recent belief is that functional problems in the striatum of schizophrenic patients are not significant enough to seriously impair procedural learning, however, research shows that the impairment will be significant enough to cause problems improving performance on a task between practice intervals. [55]
Drugs
editOverall, research concerning the effects of drugs on procedural memory is still limited. This limitation stems from the fact that procedural memory is implicit and thus more difficult to test, as opposed to declarative memory which is more pronounced and thus easier memory system to use for determining the effects of an observed drug.
Alcohol
editWhile the effects of Alcohol have been studied immensely, even with respect to memory, there is limited research examining the effects of alcohol on procedural memory. Research conducted by Pitel A. L. et al. suggests that alcoholism impairs the ability to acquire semantic concepts. In this study, while semantic concepts were understood, procedural memory was often not automated. A potential reason for this finding is that poor learning strategies are used by alcoholics compared to non-alcoholics.[56]
Cocaine
editIt is evident that long-term Cocaine abuse alters brain structures. Research has shown that the brain structures that are immediately affected by long-term cocaine abuse include: cerebral hypoperfusion in the frontal, periventricular and temporal-parietal.[57] These structures playing a role in memory various memory systems. Furthermore, the drug cocaine elicits its desirable effects by blocking the DRD1 dopamine receptors in the striatum resulting in increased dopamine levels in the brain.[57] It is important to note that these receptors are important for the consolidation of procedural memory. These increased dopamine levels in the brain resultant of cocaine use is similar to the increased dopamine levels in the brain found in schizophrenic's.[58] Studies have compared the common memory deficits caused by both cases to further understand the neural networks of procedural memory.To learn more about the effects of dopamine and it's role in schizophrenia see: dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia. Studies using rats have shown that when rats are administered trace amounts of cocaine their procedural memory systems are negatively impacted. Specifically, the rats are unable to effectively consolidate motor skill learning. [59]. With cocaine abuse being associated with poor procedural learning, research has shown that abstinence from cocaine is associated with sustained improvement of motor skill learning (Wilfred et al).
Psychostimulants
editMost psychostimulants work by inhibiting dopamine receptors in hope to elicit focus or pleasure. The usage of psychostimulants has become more widespread in the medical world for treating conditions like ADHD. Psychostimulants have been shown to be used more frequently today amongst students and other social demographics as a means to study more efficiently or have been abused for their pleasurable side effects.[60] Research suggests that when not abused, psychostimulants aid in the acquisition of procedural learning. Studies have shown that psychostimulants like d-amphetamine facilitates lower response times and increased procedural learning when compared to control participants and participants who have been administered the antipsychotic haloperidol on procedural learning tasks [61]. While improvements in procedural memory were evident when participants were administered traces of psychostimulants, many researchers have found that procedural memory is hampered when psychostimulants are abused.[62] This introduces the idea that for optimal procedural learning, dopamine levels must be balanced.
Sleep
editPractice is clearly an important process for learning and perfecting a new skill. With over 40 years of research, it is well established in both humans and animals that the formation of all forms of memory are greatly enhanced during the brain-state of sleep. Furthermore, with humans, sleep has been consistently shown to aid in the development of procedural knowledge by the ongoing process of memory consolidation, especially when sleep soon follows the initial phase of memory acquisition[63][64][65][66][67]. Memory consolidation is a process that transforms novel memories from a relatively fragile state to a more robust and stable condition. For a long time it was believed that the consolidation of procedural memories took place solely as a function of time,[68][69] but more recent studies suggest, that for certain forms of learning, the consolidation process is exclusively enhanced during periods of sleep[70]. However, it is important to note that not just any type of sleep is sufficient to improve procedural memory and performance on subsequent procedural tasks. In fact, within the domain of motor skill, there is evidence showing that no improvement on tasks is shown following a short, non-rapid eye movement (NREM; stages 2-4) sleep, such as a nap[71]. REM sleep following a period of slow-wave sleep (SWS; combined stage 3 and 4 and the deepest form of NREM sleep), has shown to be the most beneficial type of sleep for procedural memory enhancement, especially when it takes place immediately after the initial acquisition of a skill. So essentially, a full night (or day) of uninterrupted sleep soon after learning a skill will allow for the most memory consolidation possible. Furthermore, if REM sleep is disrupted, there is no gain in procedural performance shown[72]. However, equal improvement will take place whether the sleep after practice was at night or during the daytime, as long as SWS is followed by REM sleep. It has also been shown that the enhancement in memory is specific to the learned stimulus (ie, learning a running technique will not cross over to improvements in biking performance)[73]. Some examples of tasks in which subjects performance improved following REM sleep periods are: the Wff ’n Proof Task [74][75][76]; the Tower of Hanoi[77]; and the Mirror Tracing Task[78].
Whether a skill is learned explicitly (with attention) or implicitly, each plays a role in the offline consolidation effect. Research suggests that explicit awareness and understanding of the skill being learned during the acquisition process greatly improves the consolidation of procedural memories during sleep [79]. This finding is not surprising, as it is widely accepted that intention and awareness at time of learning enhances the acquisition of most forms memory.
Athletic procedures
editAthletic performance
editTo acquire an appreciation for the application of procedural memory with regard to athletic performance, Yogi Berra had once posed the rhetorical question:
- "How can you think and hit at the same time?" Yogi Berra
Golf Putt
edit- Judge the line of the ball
- Judge the grain of the turf
- Judge the distance and angle to the hole
- Image the ball going into the hole
- Position the ball somewhere between the center of your feet. You should be able to look straight down on top of the ball
- Align shoulders, hips, knees, and feet parallel and to the left of the target (e.g., image railroad tracks from the ball to the cup—feet outside the tracks, the ball in the middle)
- Grip—thumbs should be pointed straight down, palms facing each other, a light grip
- Posture—stand tall enough so that if you were to practice putting for 30 minutes you would not experience a stiff or sore back
- Arms—should hang naturally and be relaxed
- Hands—should be relative to ball position. Hands should be slightly in front of the ball
- Head position—eyes should be positioned directly over the ball
- Weight—distribute weight evenly, about 50-50, or with a little more weight on the left foot
- Backswing—swing the club straight back. The distance back that the club goes must equal the through stroke distance
- Stroke—the club must accelerate through the ball. Finish with the "face" of the club head pointing directly at the target
- Length of the stroke—it is better to err to a shorter more compact stroke rather than a longer stroke
- Stroke direction—straight back and straight through
- Stroke rhythm—not too fast and not too slow
- Keep head and lower body stationary throughout stroke and swing with the arms
- Wrists—should not break during the stroke
- Arms and shoulders—should do most of the work
- Head/trunk/hips/legs—should remain still during the stroke
- Watch the ball go into the hole [17]
See also
editFootnotes
edit- ^ Bullemer, P., Nissen, MJ., Willingham, D.B. (1989). On the Development of Procedural Knowledge. Journal of Experiemental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. 15(6); 1047-1060.
- ^ Squire, L.R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 82; 171-177.
- ^ a b Zimbardo, P. G., and Gerring, R. J. (1999). Psychology and life. (15th ed.).New York: Longman.
- ^ a b c d e f Fitts, P. M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 381-391
- ^ a b c d e f Fitts, P. M., Posner, M. I. (1967). Human Performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole
- ^ Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55–81.
- ^ Starkes, J. L., & Deakin, J. (1984). Perception in sport: A cognitive approach to skilled performance. In W. F. Straub & J. M. Williams (Eds.), Cognitive sport psychology (pp. 115–128). Lansing, MI: Sport Science Associates.
- ^ Chi, M. T., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5, 121–152.
- ^ Tenenbaum, G., & Bar-Eli, M. (1993). Decision-making in sport: A cognitive perspective. In R. N. Singer, M. Murphey, & L. K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology (pp. 171–192). New York: Macmillan.
- ^ a b Beilock S.L., Carr T.H.,MacMahon C., & Starkes J.L., (2002) When Paying Attention Becomes Counterproductive: Impact of Divided Versus Skill-Focused Attention on Novice and Experienced Performance of Sensorimotor Skills Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, Vol. 8, No. 1, 6–16
- ^ Anderson, J. R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
- ^ Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- ^ Proctor, R. W., & Dutta, A. (1995). Skill acquisition and human performance. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
- ^ Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- ^ Langer, E., & Imber, G. (1979). When practice makes imperfect: Debilitating effects of overlearning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2014–2024.
- ^ Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of a cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369-406.
- ^ a b c d e f Beilock, S.L., & Carr, T. (2001). On the Fragility of Skilled Performance: What Governs Choking Under Pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Vol. 130. No. 4. 701-725
- ^ Lewis, B., & Linder, D. (1997). Thinking about choking? Attentional processes and paradoxical performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 937-944.
- ^ Kimble, G. A., & Perlmuter, L. C. (1970). The problem of volition. Psychological Review, 77, 361
- ^ Langer, E., & Imber, G. (1979). When practice makes imperfect: Debilitating effects of overlearning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2014-2024.
- ^ Masters, R. S. (1992). Knowledge, knerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 343-358.
- ^ http://www.ctvolympics.ca/video/index.html?assetid=993f323f-81b4-408a-b20c-9efcfb161fa2
- ^ Alexander GE, Crutcher MD. Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits; neural substrates of parallel processing. Trends Neurosci. 1990; 13; 266-271
- ^ Haber SN, Fudge JL, McFarland NR. Striatonigrostriatal pathways in primates form an ascending spiral from the shell to the dorsolateral striatum. J. Neurosci. 2000; 20; 2369-2382
- ^ Parent A. Extrinsic connections of the basal ganglia. Trends in Neurosci. 1990; 13; 254-258
- ^ Smith, Y., Raju, D. V., Pare, J. F., Sidibe, M., 2004. The thalamostriatal system: a highly specific network of the basal ganglia circuitry. Trends Neurosci 27, 520–527.
- ^ Zhou FM, Wilson CJ, Dani JA. Cholinergic Interneuron characteristics and nicotinic properties in the striatum. J. Neurobiol. 2002; 53; 590-605
- ^ Kreitzer AC (2009). Physiology and pharmacology of striatal neurons. Rev Neurosci. 2009;32:127-47.
- ^ Saywell N, Taylor D (2008) The role of the cerebellum in procedural learning – are there implications for physiotherapists’ clinical practice?. Physiother Theory practice. 2008- Oct; 24 (5); 321 - 8
- ^ Nagao S, Kitazawa H. (2008) Role of the cerebellum in the acquisition and consolidation of motor memory. Brain Nerve 2008-Jul; 60 (7); 783-90
- ^ S.Y. Shu, X.M. Bao, C.Zhang, S.X. Li, W.Y. Chan, and D. Yew (2000) A New Subdivision, Marginal Division, in the Neostriatum of the Monkey Brain. Biomedical and Life Sciences. Volume 25, Number 2
- ^ Si Yun Shu, Xin Min Bao,, Qun Ning, Yong Ming Wu, Jun Wang, Brian E. Leonard (2003) New component of the limbic system; Marginal division of the neostriatum that links the limbic system to the basal nucleus of Meynert. Journal of Neuroscience Research. Vol. 71 No.5 Pg 751-757
- ^ Mizumori SJ, Puryear CB, Martig AK. (2009). Basal ganglia contributions to adaptive navigation. Behav Brain Res. 2009 Apr 12;199(1):32-42. Epub 2008 Nov 14
- ^ Zellner MR, Rinaldi R. (2009) How conditioned stimuli acquire the ability to activate VTA dopamine cells; A proposed neurobiological component of reward-related learning. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. Nov 12 2009
- ^ Pittenger C, Fasano S, Mazzocchi-Jones D, Dunnett SB, Kandel ER, Brambilla R. (2006) Impaired bidirectional synaptic plasticity and procedural memory formation in striatum-specific cAMP response element-binding protein-deficient mice. J Neurosci. 2006 Mar 8;26(10):2808-13.
- ^ van Gorp et al. (1999). Declarative and Procedural Memory Functioning in Abstinent Cocaine Abusers. Archives of general psychiatry. 56(1);85.
- ^ a b Allen, J.S., Anderson, S.W., Castro-Caldas, A., Cavaco, S., Damasio, H. (2004). The scope of preserved procedural memory in amnesia. Brain Advance Access. 10.1093.
- ^ Dotto, L. (1996). Sleep Stages, Memory and Learning. Canadian Medical Association.154(8)
- ^ Balota, D.A., Connor, L.T.,Ferraro, F.R. (1993). Implicit Memory and the Formation of New Associations in Nondemented Parkinson’s Disease Individuals and Individuals with Senile Dementia of the Alzheimer Type: A Serial Reaction Time (SRT) Investigation. Brain and Cognition. 21; 163-180.
- ^ a b c d Corkin, S., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Growdon, J. H., Mickel, S. F. (1993). Intact Acquisition and Long-Term Retention of Mirror-Tracing Skill in Alzheimer’s Disease and in Global Amnesia. Behavioural Neuroscience. 107(6); 899-910.
- ^ a b Packard, M.G., Poldrack, R.A.(2003). Competition among multiple memory systems: converging evidence from animal and human brain studies. Neuropsychologia. 41; 245-251
- ^ Dere, A. Zlomuzica, D. L.A. Ruocco, T. Watanabe, A.G. Sadile, J.P. Huston and M.A. De Souza-Silva. (2008) Episodic-like and procedural memory impairments in histamine H1 Receptor knockout mice coincide with changes in acetylcholine esterase activity in the hippocampus and dopamine turnover in the cerebellum. Neuroscience, volume 157, Issue 3, 2 December 2008, Pages 532-541.
- ^ Marsh R, Alexander GM, Packard MG, Zhu H, Peterson BS. (2005) Perceptual-motor skill learning in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Evidence for multiple procedural learning and memory systems. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43(10):1456-65. Epub 2005 Feb 1.
- ^ Reger M, Welsh R, Razani J, Martin DJ, Boone Kb. A meta-analysis of the neuropsychological sequelae of HIV infection. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2002; 8;410-424
- ^ Chang L, Lee PL, Yiannoutsos CT, Ernst T, Marra CM, Richards T, et al. (2004) A multicenter in vivo proton-MRS study of HIV-associated dementia and its relationship to age. Neuroimage.(23): 1336-1347
- ^ Gonzalez R, Jacobus J, Amatya AK, Quartana PJ, Vassileva J, Martin EM (2008) Deficits in complex motor functions, despite no evidence of procedural learning deficits, among HIV+ individuals with history of substance dependence. Neuropsychology. Nov 2008;22(6):776-86.
- ^ Sprengelmeyer R, Canavan AG, Lange HW, Hömberg V (1995) Associative learning in degenerative neostriatal disorders: contrasts in explicit and implicit remembering between Parkinson's and Huntington's diseases. Mov Disord. 1995 Jan;10(1):51-65.
- ^ Saint-Cyr JA, Taylor AE, Lang AE. (1988) Procedural learning and neostriatal dysfunction in man. Brain. 1988 Aug;111 ( Pt 4):941-59.
- ^ Roth RM, Baribeau J, Milovan D, O'Connor K, Todorov C.(2004) Procedural and declarative memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder.J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004 Sep;10(5):647-54
- ^ Sarazin M, Deweer B, Pillon B, Merkl A, Dubois B. (2001) Procedural learning and Parkinson disease: implication of striato-frontal loops. Rev Neurol. 2001 Dec;157(12):1513-8.
- ^ Muslimovic D, Post B, Speelman JD, Schmand B.(2007) Motor procedural learning in Parkinson's disease. Brain. 2007 Nov;130 (Pt 11):2887-97. Epub 2007 Sep 13.
- ^ Sarazin M, Deweer B, Merkl A, Von Poser N, Pillon B, Dubois B. (2002) Procedural learning and striatofrontal dysfunction in Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2002 Mar;17(2):265-73
- ^ Lang DJ, Kopala lC, Smith GN, et al (1999) MRI study of basal ganglia volumes in drug-naive first-episode patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1999; 36:202.
- ^ A Chatterjee, M Chakos, A Koreen, S Geisler, B Sheitman, M Woerner, JM Kane J Alvir and Ja (1995). Prevalence and clinical correlates of extrapyramidal signs and spontaneous dyskinesia in never-medicated schizophrenic patients. Am J Psychiatry. 1995 Dec; 152 (12); 1724-9.
- ^ Schérer H, Stip E, Paquet F, Bédard MA (2003). Mild procedural learning disturbances in neuroleptic-naive patients with schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2003 Winter;15(1):58-63.
- ^ Pitel A. L., Witkowski T., Vabret F., Guillery-Girard B., Desgranges B., Eustache F., Beaunieux H. (2007). Effect of episodic and working memory impairments on semantic and cognitive procedural learning at alcohol treatment entry.Alcohol Clin Exp Res, 31(2), 238-48.
- ^ a b Strickland, T. L., Mena, I., Villanueva-Meyer, J., Miller B. L., Cummings, J., Mehringer, C. M., Satz, P., & Myers, H. (1993). Cerebral perfusion and neuropsychological consequences of chronic cocaine use. American Neuropsychiatric Association, 5, 419-427.
- ^ Serper, M. R., Bermanc, A., Copersinoa, M. L., Choub, J. C. Y., Richarmea, D., and Cancrob, R. (2000).Learning and memory impairment in cocaine-dependent and comorbid schizophrenic patients.Psychiatry Research, 93(1), 21-32
- ^ Willuhn I, Steiner H. (2008) Motor-skill learning in a novel running-wheel task is dependent on D1 dopamine receptors in the striatum. Neuroscience, April 22; 153 (1); 249-58. Epub 2008 Feb 6.
- ^ McCabe, S. E., Knight, J. R., Teter, C. J., Wechsler, H. (2004). Non-medical use of prescription stimulants among US college students: prevalence and correlates from anational survey. Research Report.
- ^ Kumari, V., Gray, J.A., Corr, P.J., Mulligan, O.F., Cotter, P.A., Checkley, S.A. (1997). Effects of acute administration of d-amphetamine and haloperidol on procedural learning in man. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 129(3); 271-276
- ^ Toomey, R., Lyons, M. J., Eisen, S. A., Xian, Hong., Chantarujikapong, Sunanta., Seidman, L. J., Faraone, S., Tsuang, M. T. (2003). A Twin Study of the Neuropsychological Consequences of Stimulant Abuse. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 60, 303-310.
- ^ Karni, A., Tanne, D., Rubenstein, B.S., Askenasy, J.J., and Sagi, D. 1994. Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill. Science 265: 679–682.
- ^ Gais, S., Plihal, W., Wagner, U., and Born, J. 2000. Early sleep triggers memory for early visual discrimination skills. Nat. Neurosci.3: 1335–1339.
- ^ Stickgold, R., James, L., and Hobson, J.A. 2000a. Visual discrimination learning requires sleep after training. Nat. Neurosci. 3: 1237–1238.
- ^ Stickgold, R., Whidbee, D., Schirmer, B., Patel, V., and Hobson, J.A.2000b. Visual discrimination task improvement: A multi-step process occurring during sleep. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12: 246–254.
- ^ Walker, M.P., Brakefield, T., Morgan, A., Hobson, J.A., and Stickgold, R.2002. Practice with sleep makes perfect: Sleep dependent motor skill learning. Neuron 35: 205–211.
- ^ Brashers-Krug, T., Shadmehr, R., and Bizzi, E. 1996. Consolidation in human motor memory. Nature 382: 252–255.
- ^ McGaugh, J.L. 2000. Memory—A century of consolidation. Science 287: 248–251.
- ^ Fischer, S., Hallschmid, M., Elsner, A.L., and Born, J. 2002. Sleep forms memory for finger skills. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 99: 11987–11991.
- ^ Siegel, J. M. (2001). The REM sleep-memory consolidation hypothesis.Science, 294(5544), 1058–1063.
- ^ Karni, A., Meyer, G., Rey-Hipolito, C., Jezzard, P., Adams, M.M., Turner, R., and Ungerleider, L.G. 1998. The acquisition of skilled motor performance: Fast and slow experience-driven changes in primarymotor cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 95: 861–868.
- ^ Mednick, S.C. et al. (2003) Sleep-dependent learning: a nap is as good as a night. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 697–698.
- ^ Smith C. REM sleep and learning: some recent findings. In: Moffit A, Kramer M, Hoffman H, editors. The functions of dreaming. Albany:SUNY; 1993.
- ^ Smith C, Fazekas A. Amount of REM sleep and Stage 2 sleep required for efficient learning. Sleep Res 1997;26:690.
- ^ Smith C, Weeden K. Post training REMs coincident auditory stimulation enhances memory in humans. Psychiatr J Univ Ott 1990;15(2):85–90.
- ^ Smith CT, Nixon MR, Nader RS. Post training increases in REM sleep intensity implicate REM sleep in memory processing and provide a biological marker of learning potential. Learn Mem 2004;11(6):714–9.
- ^ Conway J, Smith C. REM sleep and learning in humans: a sensitivity to specific types of learning tasks. In: Proceedings of the 12th Congress of the European Sleep Research Society. 1994.
- ^ Robertson, E.M. et al. (2004) Awareness modifies skill-learning benefits of sleep. Curr. Biol. 14, 208–212.