This article includes a list of general references, but it lacks sufficient corresponding inline citations. (August 2010) |
Part of a series on |
Reading |
---|
The initial teaching alphabet (i.t.a.) is a teaching orthography for English consisting of a variation of the Latin alphabet and a spelling system which was developed during the late 1950s in the United Kingdom by Sir James Pitman (the grandson of Sir Isaac Pitman, inventor of a system of shorthand) . It was intended to be a practical simplified writing system which could be used to teach English-speaking children to read & write more easily than by using traditional orthography (t.o.). After children had learnt to read using i.t.a, they would transition their newly acquired literacy skills to t.o. and learn standard English spelling.
It achieved a degree of popularity through the English speaking world during the 1960s & 1970s, at its peak approx. 10%[1][2] of children in the United States and the United Kingdom were learning to read & write using the i.t.a. which equated to many millions of children over the two decades[1][2]. This enabled a considerable level of research & scientific study to be carried out on teaching literacy using very large samples of pupils and the i.t.a. as a benchmark orthography. The popularity of the i.t.a. resulted from its generally superior literacy outcomes[3] in teaching children literacy but at the expense of an increased financial and organisational burden[4] from using two orthographies instead of one.
Etymology
editThe i.t.a. was an abbreviation of orthography's final name, the initial teaching alphabet. Neither the name or the abbreviation used capitalisation in respect to the fact the orthography deliberately omitted the use of capital letters in order to simplify the initial learning of literacy.
It was also previously known as The Augmented Latin Alphabet and originally as the The Ehrhardt Augmented Roman Alphabet (the Monotype Corporation of Salfords, Surrey had kindly extended their Ehrhardt (typeface) to include the new i.t.a. characters[5] and so were accredited in the original name).
Technically, the i.t.a. should have been known as the initial teaching orthography but since most people would not know what an orthography was, it was simplified to the initial teaching alphabet.
Background to the i.t.a.
editThe i.t.a. was developed in the United Kingdom in the 1950s, the background to its creation is described.
Literacy
editAfter World War II, the Ministry of Education measured and acknowledged the on-going literacy problem in the UK[6], this showed one in four children reaching age fifteen with deficient literacy levels. As similar results were seen throughout the English speaking world, suspicion fell on the irregularity of the English language as the cause. Arguments for English spelling reform to improve literacy were made by influential thinkers such as George Bernard Shaw, Dr Montefiore Follick (MP) and Sir James Pitman (MP).
Category | 1948 | 1952 | 1956 |
---|---|---|---|
Superior | 9% | 9% | 9% |
Average (+) | 34% | 39% | 43% |
Average (-) | 27% | 22% | 23% |
Backward | 24% | 25% | 21% |
Semi-Literate | 5% | 4% | 4% |
Illiterate | 1% | 1% | 0% |
TOTAL | 100% | 100% | 100% |
In post-war Britain, the choice of method for teaching literacy was delegated by the government to local education authorities who in turn often delegated to head teachers, in addition there was a diverse variety of early education schools ranging from church schools to state schools to independent schools. The government through the Ministry of Education employed a light touch whereby they did not explicitly interfere in the running of schools but attempted to simply influence them, albeit the Ministry did enforce a minimum set of teaching standards through Her Majesty's Schools Inspectorate. The Ministry of Education used circulars to provide advice and guidance to schools which are all recorded at the National Archives.
This decentralised schooling system was both the reason the i.t.a. initially took hold in early 1960s and also the reason it was not universally adopted.
Teaching methods
editThe type of method for English reading instruction has swung over time between whole language approaches and phonics based methods[8]. The i.t.a. developed during the post war years which was a transition period from whole language to phonics, at the time there were three broad methods in operation: -
METHOD | POPULARITY | DESCRIPTION |
---|---|---|
Look & Say + Phonics | Most popular | Known as a hybrid 'eclectic' approach[9], this consisted of initially using Look-and-Say with basal readers for the most common words then teaching switched to using phonics for more advanced words as the limits of the whole-of-language method was reached. This 'eclectic' approach had developed because prior research had shown that look-and-say allowed children to initially learn more quickly before the limitations of using memory forced the use of of phonics to continue reading development. |
Phonics | Second most popular | Here phonics was used from the outset, it was recognised that learning was initially slower but pupils were expected to catch-up. |
Progressive | Least popular | The progressive method was based on modern thinking whereby the priority was to instigate a desire in reading by introducing children to exciting and imaginative literature rather than using structured basal readers. |
The other major factor in teaching literacy was timing. The two schools of thought were to either delay the start of reading instruction until the child had developed further with the aim of then progressing quickly or to start early and initially progress slowly but to give the child a head-start.
Teaching materials
editReading Schemes Common in Great Britain in 1960s | ||
---|---|---|
The Happy Venture Readers | Janet & John (most popular) | The Beacon Readers |
Vanguard Readers | The Pilot Reading Scheme | Gay Way Readers |
The McKee Readers | John & Mary, & Mac & Tosh | The Royal Road Readers |
In addition, a minority of teachers rejected these schemes and developed their own scheme based on a variety of books of their own choosing, and their own exercises and word lists.
Government
editAfter the war, school leaving age was increased to 15 in 1947[10], also the United Kingdom suffered a post-war baby boom. The result of these two factors was a considerable increase in the number of children at school with the school population rising from 5 to 7 million in the post-war years[11]. This put a crisis level strain on the school infrastructure with over-flowing classes, inadequate facilities and severe pressure on education budgets.[12] With the government preoccupied and financially constrained, it did not want to be distracted by the i.t.a. and had no funding to spare so it took a hands-off approach to the i.t.a.
Origins of the i.t.a.
editParliamentary agitation
editAfter World War II, Dr Montefiore Follick (MP) and Sir James Pitman (MP) worked together to promote English orthographic reform in Parliament. Their first private members' bill proposed forming a committee for spelling reform, but it was defeated at the second reading. Their second bill, which proposed a simplified spelling system for early childhood education, passed through the committee stage and won the vote for the second reading. This embarrassing parliamentary victory over the Winston Churchill government allowed Pitman to negotiate with the Education Minister, Miss Florence Horsbrugh MP, to withdraw the bill in return for allowing local education authorities and research institutes to trial a new teaching orthography. Although it needed to be self-funded and self-organised, Pitman had the resources for the orthographic experiment through his ownership of the Pitman Printing Press.
The protagonists
editFollick and Pitman were an unlikely political partnership, as they were from opposite ends of the political spectrum. Follick was a firebrand socialist Labour MP, and Pitman was a wealthy Conservative MP. They united in a common cause against the leadership of their respective parties and the establishment, in general, to prioritise children's needs and improve their literacy. They won their political campaign by criticising the status quo but only providing vague ideas on an alternative, preventing counter-criticism on the specifics. They were well aware of the centuries-old failures to enact orthographic reform and were keen to avoid repeating previous mistakes.
The stalemate
editThe trial required a simplified English orthography, but Follick and Pitman had differing opinions on the best approach. Follick favoured a phonetically pure and completely simplified orthography, with the only consideration being the ease with which children can learn to read, as he ambitiously believed it would provide the evidence needed to initiate spelling reform. On the other hand, Pitman pragmatically felt that spelling reform was unlikely to happen; however, children's literacy outcomes could still be significantly improved through learning to read with a simplified orthography, but this made the transition back to traditional English orthography a critical factor in the new design. Both produced draft orthographies, and in the late 1950s, they debated to a stalemate whether to adopt Pitman's pragmatic or Follick's ambitions approach.
Parliamentary agitation
editIn the immediate post war years, Dr. Montefiore Follick (MP) with the backing of Sir James Pitman (MP), attempted to advance the cause of English orthographic reform in the UK Parliament. The first private members bill was to form a committee for spelling reform but it was defeated on 2nd reading[13]. This was followed by a more restrained second bill to trial a simplified spelling system just for the early teaching of children, this won the vote for a 2nd reading and passed through the committee stage[14], which embarrassed the Winston Churchill government. Pitman subsequently negotiated with the Education minister (Miss Florence Horsbrugh MP) and it was agreed that Follick would withdraw the bill, in return the minister publicly stated the government would not stand in the way of a trial by local education authorities & research institutes, indeed the minister wished this endeavour her best wishes. This conceded blessing was the green light for the trial to take place albeit it would need to be self-funded and self-organised, which allowed Pitman rather than the government to shape the course of events because Pitman was both wealthy, influential and had ownership of the Pitman Printing Press[15] so he possessed the resources for an orthographic experiment which the government then lacked.
Protagonists
editFollick and Pitman had won their political campaign against the odds by logically & factually criticizing the status quo but only providing general principles on an alternative, this prevented specific counter criticism on the detail and is a well renowned political tactic[16]. The duo formed a formidable political partnership as they were from opposite ends of the political spectrum (Follick was a socialist Labour MP & Pitman a wealthy Conservative MP) yet they were united in a common cause against the leadership in both parties and the establishment in general. They believed that their goals, to help children and place their needs as a priority, was morally and ethically righteous. Their natures also complimented each other in that Follick was a firebrand and leader of the cause whilst Pitman was a negotiator and pragmatist, it is clear they forged a strong friendship through their campaigns.
The trial needed a simplified English orthography but there were two options. The first was a phonetically pure and completely simplified orthography with no consideration for the transition of the children back to t.o. This option was favoured by Follick who was committed to the ultimate prize of orthographic reform, his position was to maximise the improvement in children's literacy which would provide the overwhelming evidence needed to initiate spelling reform. Pitman reasoned that the centuries old goal of spelling reform would likely remain out of reach no matter what the findings of the trial, but children's literacy outcomes could still be significantly improved by learning to read with a simplified orthography, this would only work if the child could effortlessly transition back to t.o. A substantial level of simplification was still possible within this design constraint. Both Pitman and Follick produced draft orthographies and a hiatus set in during the later half of the '50s whilst this debate occurred.
Launch of the i.t.a.
editThe impasse was broken with the death of Monte on the 10th Dec 1958[19], Pitman effectively became the leading proponent of spelling reform and published his orthography "The Ehrhardt Augmented Roman Alphabet" on 29th May 1959 in the Times Educational Supplement.[20] Pitman orchestrated a public show of support for the trial from the new education minister, the teaching unions, the education committees, key universities and educational research institutes[21]. A governance committee was formed to steer the experiment and the nation's principal educational research institute, the London xxxxx spun up the Reading Research Unit to carry out the trial under the leadership of Dr John Downing. Articles in the educational press and lectures raised awareness leading to the nationwide distribution of a pamphlet to all local education authorities, educators, head teachers, publishers and early learning teachers explaining the trial and seeking support for the endevour.
Governance committee members with I.J. Pitman[22] | ||
---|---|---|
H.L. Elvin | W.R. Niblett | W.D. Wall |
P.E. Vernon | Joyce M. Morris | Cyril Burt |
Trial of the i.t.a.
editBy September 1960, Downing had meticulously organised the trial on a scientific basis using the standard Janet & John Basel readers which were transliterated into the i.t.a. The trial was controlled by identifying factors such as socio-economic status and pairing alike schools together so one school participated in the experiment whilst the twin taught in the same way but using t.o., hence allowing for fair comparison. Unfortunately, most schools considered the use of an interim orthography to be a bizarre idea and so happily volunteered to be controls but very few volunteered to join the i.t.a. experiment. This resulted in the schools trialling the i.t.a. to be at a disadvantage to the control schools: -
- The i.t.a. class sizes were larger
- The i.t.a. children were younger
- The i.t.a. schools had worst buildings & classroom facilities
- There was a shortage of i.t.a. books in the book corner
- The i.t.a. teachers had no experience and little training of using the i.t.a.
The details of the trial were[23]: -
Academic Years | ||
---|---|---|
1961 | 1962 | 1963 |
Classes | Pupils | |
---|---|---|
Experimental Groups | 41 | 873 |
Control Groups | 41 | 873 |
TOTAL | 82 | 1746 |
Findings of the i.t.a. trial
editThe measured results of the three year trial showed a clear superiority of using i.t.a. over t.o.[24]: -
- Pupils using i.t.a. made more rapid progress through the basal reader series
- Pupils who learnt through i.t.a. achieved higher scores on reading tests
- Pupils who learnt through i.t.a. wrote longer compositions
- Pupils who learnt through i.t.a. attained a more extensive vocabulary
Progress through the identical Basel reader series in i.t.a. aendeavour(t.o.)[25]
Primer No. | % of i.t.a. pupils | % of traditional orthography pupils |
---|---|---|
< Book 2 | ||
= Book 3 | ||
= Book 4 | ||
= Book 5 | ||
> Book 5 |
After the pupils transitioned to t.o., reading tests showed the advantages persisted[26]: -
Graphs are unavailable due to technical issues. There is more info on Phabricator and on MediaWiki.org. |
Outcome of the i.t.a. trial
editAs the i.t.a. trial was publicized, it caused a commotion in the press and media with large numbers of visitors including journalists descending on the schools trialing the i.t.a. By 1963, the initial trial by John Downing published its findings, socialising the results and recommending the use of the i.t.a. The resulting positive media coverage spread throughout the English speaking world, especially to the USA, where a large number of trials were carried out to validate the findings in Great Britain. With the release of the i.t.a. in 1963, a rapid adoption by schools and publishers then took place, by 1967, the schools council measured the level of adoption and calculated 9.7% of schools in Great Britain had adopted the i.t.a. but this adoption was still expanding at this point and is likely to have peaked around the turn of the decade (c. 1970).
The aim of the i.t.a. was to act as an interim orthography to enable children to learn to read and write easier than by using t.o. Using an interim orthography meant there were two steps in learning: -
- Step (1) - becoming literate in the interim orthography
- Step (2) - transitioning literacy to t.o.
In comparison, normally children became literate in one-step, direct to t.o. There was no point in using an interim orthography unless Step (1) + Step (2) were significantly more successful than one-step learning.
Bearing this in mind, there were two design options for the new interim orthography: -
- Option (1) - design a phonetically pure and completely regular orthography which would be exceptionally quick to learn and give no consideration to transition to t.o.
- Option (2) - give consideration to transition to t.o. by compromising on the phonetic purity and regularity of the interim orthography
An important factor used for the decision on which design option to take was the choice of teaching method. Teaching through the use of phonics naturally led to a preference for option (1) whereas option (2) also allowed the design to be orientated towards whole-of-word (known as 'look & say' at that time).
Sir James Pitman favoured option (2) and positioned the i.t.a. as an agnostic medium for teaching where either a phonics teaching method or the prevalent 'look & say' method could be used.
Mechanism for transition
editAs the transition from interim orthography to t.o. was a design priority, so a mechanism was needed to allow easy transition. This came from educational psychology where experimentation had discovered an effect called the 'top coast-line' effect which was also used as a basis of whole-of-word methods, which espoused the importance of word-shapes in teaching to read. The experimental observation was that when lines of words were masked at the bottom then they remained easy to decipher by the fluent reader whereas those masked at the top became difficult to read.[28]
The i.t.a. was designed so as to retain the top word shape with traditional English orthography as far as possible. The result of this design decision is that i.t.a. retained a considerable level of irregularity so it was more difficult to learn than needed to be whereas transition to traditional English was found to be effortless.
Details
editThe i.t.a. originally had 43 symbols, which was expanded to 44, then 45. Each symbol predominantly represented a single English sound (including affricates and diphthongs), but there were complications due to the desire to avoid making the i.t.a. needlessly different from standard English spelling (which would make the transition from the i.t.a. to standard spelling more difficult), and in order to neutrally represent several English pronunciations or dialects. In particular, there was no separate i.t.a. symbol for the English unstressed schwa sound [ə], and schwa was written with the same letters used to write full vowel sounds. There were also several different ways of writing unstressed [ɪ]/[i] and consonants palatalized to [tʃ], [dʒ], [ʃ], [ʒ] by suffixes. Consonants written by double letters or "ck", "tch" etc. sequences in standard spelling were written with multiple symbols in i.t.a.
The i.t.a. symbol set includes joined letters (typographical ligatures) to replace the two-letter digraphs "wh", "sh", and "ch" of conventional writing, and also ligatures for most of the long vowels. There are two distinct ligatures for the voiced and unvoiced "th" sounds in English, and a special merged letter for "ng" resembling ŋ with a loop. There is a variant of the "r" to end syllables, which is silent in non-rhotic accents like Received Pronunciation but not in rhotic accents like General American and Scots English (this was the 44th symbol added to the i.t.a.).
There are two English sounds which each have more than one i.t.a. letter whose main function is to write them. So whether the sound [k] is written with the letters "c" or "k" in i.t.a. depends on the way the sound is written in standard English spelling, as also whether the sound [z] is written with the ordinary "z" letter or with a special backwards "z" letter (which replaces the "s" of standard spelling where it represents a voiced sound, and which visually resembles an angular form of the letter "s"). The backwards "z" occurs prominently in many plural forms of nouns and third-person singular present forms of verbs (including is).
Each of the i.t.a. letters has a name, the pronunciation of which includes the sound that the character stands for. For example, the name of the backwards "z" letter is "zess".
A special typeface was created for the i.t.a., whose characters were all lower case (its letter forms were based on Didone types such as Monotype Modern and Century Schoolbook). Where capital letters are used in standard spelling, the i.t.a. simply used larger versions of the same lower-case characters. The following chart shows the letters of the 44-character version of the i.t.a., with the main pronunciation of each letter indicated by symbols of the International Phonetic Alphabet beneath:
Note that "d" is made more distinctively different from "b" than is usual in standard typefaces (which is possible since in the i.t.a. there is no "q").
Later a 45th symbol was added to accommodate accent variation, a form of diaphonemic writing. In the original set, a "hook a" or "two-storey a" (a) was used for the vowel in "cat" (lexical set TRAP), and a "round a" or "one-storey a" (ɑ) for the sound in "father" (lexical set PALM). But lexical set BATH (words such as "rather", "dance", and "half") patterns with PALM in some accents including Received Pronunciation, but with TRAP in others including General American. So a new character, the "half-hook a", was devised, to avoid the necessity of producing separate instructional materials for speakers of different accents.
Research into the i.t.a.
editThe largescale adoption of i.t.a. enabled a considerable level of controlled research to carried out into this subject. Professor Warburton summarised this[29] for the schools council: -
Author | Title | Date | Pupils | Method | Basal Readers | Period | Tests | Location |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bosma, R.L. | Teaching reading with i.t.a.;
a research report |
1965 | 22 | As per Basal
Teachers Guides |
Downing Readers (i.t.a.)
Various (t.o.) |
9 months | Metropolitan Achievement Test | University campus school |
Dell, G.A. | The i.t.a. approach to reading | 1967 | 54 | Phonic (i.t.a.)
Look & Say (t.o) |
Downing Readers (i.t.a.)
Janet & John Readers (t.o.) |
15 months | Schonell's Graded Word Test | Glasgow, Scotland |
Downong, J.A. | The i.t.a. symposium | 1967 | 1,746 | Look & Say | Janet & John Readers (i.t.a)
Janet & John Readers (t.o.) |
3 years | Schonell's Graded Word Test
Neale's Reading Ability Test Standish N.S. 45 Test |
United Kingdom |
Downing, J.A.
Jones, B. |
Problems of evaluating the i.t.a.;
2nd experiment |
1966 | 1,102 | Look & Say | Janet & John Readers | 3 years | Schonell's Graded Word Test
Neale's Reading Ability Test |
United Kingdom |
Fry, E. | Comparison of 3 methods of
Reading Instruction |
1966 | 390 | As per Basal
Teachers Guides |
Sheldon Readers (phonics)
Sheldon Readers (diacritical) Early to Read (i.t.a.) |
2 years | Stanford Achievement Test
Gilmore Oral Reading Test Detroit Word Recognition Test |
New Jersey. USA |
Hahn, H.T. | Relative effectiveness of 3 methods
of teaching reading |
1965 | 890 | Language Arts | Bespoke | 140 days | Stanford Achievement Test
Gilmore Oral Reading Test San Diego Inventory of Attitudes Gates Word Pronunciation Karlsen's Phonetic Words Test Fry Phonetically Regular Words |
Michigan, USA |
Harrison, M. | Instant Reading | 1964 | 436 | Look & Say | Janet & John Readers (i.t.a)
Janet & John Readers (t.o.) |
3 years | Southgate Group Reading Tests
Neale's Reading Ability Test |
Oldham, England |
Hayes, R.B.
Nemeth, J.S. |
Factors affecting learning to read | 1965 | 365 | Whole-Word
Phonics Eclectic (both) Language Arts |
Scott Foresman Readers (t.o.)
Lippincott Readers (t.o.) Phonics & Word-Power (t.o.) Downing Readers (i.t.a.) |
140 days | Stanford Achievement Test
Gilmore Oral Reading Test San Diego Inventory of Attitudes Gates Word Pronunciation Phonetic Words Oral Test |
Pennsylvania, USA |
McCracken, R.A. | Study of Early-to-Read (i.t.a.) | 1966 | 60 | As per Basal
Teachers Guides |
Early-to-Read (i.t.a.)
Ginn Basel Series (t.o.) |
2 years | Stanford Achievement Test
Stanford Reading Inventory Gray's Oral Reading Test |
Hempstead, New York |
Mazurkiewicz, A.J. | The initial teaching alphabet in
reading instruction |
1967 | 915 | Language Arts | Early-to-Read (i.t.a.)
Downing Readers (i.t.a.) Alice & Jerry (t.o.) American Book Company Language Arts Book (t.o.) |
2 years | Botel Reading Inventory
Stanford Achievement Test Gilmore Oral Reading Test Gates Word Pronunciation Karlsen's Phonetic Words Test |
Bethlehem, Pennsylvania |
Milne, A. | The Scottish i.t.a. research | 1966 | 720 | Look & Say | Janet & John Readers (both)
Stott Program Reading Kit |
3 years | Burt-Vernon Word Recognition
N.F.E.R.N.S. 45 Test |
Scotland, U.K. |
Robinson, H.M. | Effectiveness of i.t.a. as a medium
for reading instruction |
1966 | 450 | As per Basal
Teachers Guides |
Scott Foresman Readers (i.t.a.)
Scott Foresman Readers (t.o.) |
1 year | Huelsman Word Discrimination
Metropolitan Achievement Test Gray's Oral Reading Test |
New York, U.S.A. |
Shapiro, B.J. | A comparison or reading
achievement of i.t.a. versus t.o. |
1966 | 1,018 | Various | Various | 1 year | ERC Reading Mastery Test
Stanford Achievement Test |
Clevedon, Ohio |
Stewart, M.R. | Two years with i.t.a. | 1966 | 1,350 | Various | Various | 2 years | Stanford Spelling Test
California Reading Test |
Hempstead, New York |
Swales, T.D. | The attainments of children who
learned through the i.t.a. |
1966 | 198 | Look & Say | Janet & John Readers (both)
Happy Venture (t.o) |
2 years | NFER Sentence Reading Test
Daniels & Diack Spelling Test |
Staffordshire, U.K. |
Tanzyer, H.J.
Alpert, H. Sandel, L. |
Beginning reading;
effectiveness of different media |
1965 | 1,946 | As per Basal
Teachers Guides |
Early to Read (i.t.a.)
Various Basal Readers (t.o.) Fifty Library Books (both) |
2 years | Stanford Achievement Test
Detroit Word Recognition Test |
Mineola, New York |
Major findings
editThe range of literacy outcomes from using the i.t.a. varied with the majority of studies showing a significant advantage whilst a minority of studies showed a more marginal advantage or equivalence to using t.o. In no circumstances was there found to be any disadvantage to using the i.t.a.
Pupils who struggled with literacy would benefit the least from using i.t.a. Pupils who were most capable stretched their advantage when using the i.t.a.
It was critical that pupils only transitioned to t.o. when they were very fluent in reading in i.t.a. Transition from i.t.a. to t.o. was irrefutably found to be effortless
Preference | Head Teachers | Class Teachers |
---|---|---|
initial teaching alphabet (i.t.a.) | 26 | 78 |
i.t.a. (with caveats) | 2 | 6 |
traditional orthography (t.o.) | 0 | 2 |
uncertain | 1 | 1 |
TOTAL | 29 | 90 |
Professor Warburton identified the following advantages to using i.t.a. from the body of research: -
Children's Educational Outcomes
edit- Children learnt to read faster, more accurately and with greater comprehension in i.t.a. then in t.o.[32][33]
- Children developed a passion for reading, rapidly exhausting the books in the book corner whereas this rarely occurred with t.o.[34]
- Children could quickly write freely in i.t.a. using their day-to-day vocabulary whereas t.o. constrained writing to simple words so free writing was not possible[35]
- Children quickly became voracious creative writers in comparison t.o. children who generally stuck to reading[35]
- Children developed a wider spoken vocabulary and better pronunciation of spoken English[36]
- Children became more confident & independent using the i.t.a. as they swiftly dropped the dependency on adults to help them read & write[37]
- Children progressed faster in other subjects as they were better able to read the text-books necessary to progress[38]
- Children using the i.t.a. were better in arithmetic, the causal reason for this was not clearly understood at the time[39]
- Children using the i.t.a. developed a positive attitude to learning whereas children using t.o. showed frustration in learning through bad behaviour in class[40]
- Unexpectedly, children who learnt using the i.t.a. could spell better in t.o. after transition, it was speculated that the i.t.a. frame of reference allowed them to mentally use spelling rules rather than simply memorising spellings as did most children from t.o.[41]
- The number of non-readers or semi-illiterate children who had failed to learn to read properly was greatly reduced[42][43]
- any linguistic or hearing issues in children could be clearly identified from i.t.a. writing.[44][45]
Teaching Standards
edit- Teachers understanding of the written English language & its relationship to English linguistics (the foundations of reading) were significantly improved, making them better teachers[46]
- The amount of teacher time necessary to teach reading was reduced allowing more time on other subjects
- Anyone who reads can try to teach reading, the i.t.a. created a barrier to entry which prevented schools using sub-standard teachers to fill-in when necessary
- Children were less likely to be held back from having under-capable or under-trained teachers
All the disadvantages to the use of the i.t.a. were related to the organisation and cost of education, these were: -
Books
edit- Books in i.t.a. were more expensive then those in t.o.[47][48]
- It was necessary to purchase both i.t.a. & t.o. sets of books to cater for the child-by-child transition to t.o.[47][48]
- There were less children's reading books in i.t.a. than in t.o.[47][48]
- Libraries had less i.t.a. books and these tended to be on waiting lists due to a higher desire to read from i.t.a. taught children[49]
- As i.t.a. taught children had a greater desire for reading, they were more likely to pester parents for reading books causing them a financial burden[50]
- Parents struggled to locate i.t.a. children's books as they were not stocked in local bookshops and there was no internet at the time[49]
- Children at the time often purchased comics from newsagents with their pocket money but dishearteningly, none were in i.t.a.[49]
Publishing Metric | Number |
---|---|
Publishers | 10 |
Basal Reading Schemes | 5 |
Basal Reading Books | 130 |
Supplementary Book Titles | 490 |
Imports from U.S.A. | unknown |
Teacher created content | unknown |
N.B. teacher created content mainly consisted of t.o. books with i.t.a. text pasted over the t.o. text using paper glue[51]
Parental Support
edit- Some parents felt disenfranchised from the children's education because they could not easily read their i.t.a. books or help with their i.t.a. creative writing.[51]
Changing School
edit- A child forced to move from an i.t.a. school to a t.o. school (e.g. parent changes job & moves) faced a significant set-back to their education[52]
- More time & cost was needed to train i.t.a. teachers, although it was realised that this resulted in a better calibre of teachers
- Most supply teachers and volunteers could not be used to cover absences as most were unqualified to teach with the i.t.a.
Risks of using i.t.a.
editVera Southgate identified two risks to using the i.t.a. which teachers had to be cognizant of: -
Comprehension
editThe success of the i.t.a. resulted in a new problem rarely seen when using t.o. Many children were able to quickly gain independence in reading and then rapidly progressed through their reading scheme beyond their vocabulary so they would come across words they correctly read but incorrectly comprehended e.g. "the knight feasting at the banquet" could be misinterpreted as meaning "the knight is fighting at the bank". This was compounded by a lack of i.t.a. books so children exhausted the books with simpler vocabulary and self-progressed too quickly. Teachers were used to pushing children to read and suddenly faced the diametrically opposite necessity to restrain children's reading.
Transition to t.o.
editThe most important lesson for successfully using the i.t.a. was to only transition a child to t.o. once complete fluency had been achieved at which point the activity was effortless.
Exploiting i.t.a; organisation of schooling
editProf. Warburton found the use of i.t.a. was not being exploited to its full potential due to the following structural reasons: -
Leadership
editHer Majesty's Schools Inspectorate reported that only head teachers deemed as 'good' were adopting the i.t.a.[54]
Professor Warburton identified an attitude problem in many head teachers of infant schools, whereby learning to read was not deemed a prime objective of a child's education. These head teachers did not seek to monitor and improve the teaching of literacy, the reading materials were not constantly reviewed & changed instead books were old, tired and tattered, there was no attempt to recruit high calibre and experienced teachers, there was little comprehension of the different methods available let alone which one was the best but the clearest indicator of such poor leadership was the fact these heads inevitably delayed the start of reading instruction, waiting for children to develop. This was a fallacy as schools which started reading early out-paced the schools who delayed, the children never caught-up.
Junior Schools
editAlthough slow learners greatly benefited from using i.t.a.[43][42], it was important that they only transition to t.o. after they had reached fluency. If they transitioned earlier than the benefit of using i.t.a. to reduce poor illiteracy was reduced. As such a small number of children needed to continue using i.t.a. once they had moved from infant to junior schools, unfortunately many Junior schools did not have the trained teachers or materials to teach in i.t.a. or their head teachers had no inclination to use the i.t.a. There were recorded cases where infant schools stopped using the i.t.a. because their Junior school had a head teacher who did not support the i.t.a.[55]
Junior schools were also unprepared for the influx of i.t.a. taught children who were significantly better at literacy then their normal in-take, instead all children were given the same reading materials so in effect the i.t.a. children were put in a holding pattern rather than capitalising on the increased reading ability[56]. Also it became apparent that teaching English Language at Junior schools was not explicit instead children learnt through using English in other subjects, Warburton suggested that the solid foundation laid by i.t.a. was built upon through further explicit instruction.
The i.t.a. resulted in brighter children improving at a much faster rate then slower readers, which in turn resulted in a greater spread in class between the best readers and the worst readers for i.t.a. classes. Teachers were forced to phase the transition of children from i.t.a. to t.o. which considerably increased their workload as they had to teach some children in i.t.a. and others in t.o. The result was a significant strain on teachers[58]. Two broad organisational solutions were adopted to this dilemma: -
- Some larger schools reorganised their classes to stream so fluent children ready to transition were transferred to a higher t.o. class so teachers only taught in i.t.a. or t.o. but never both at the same time
- Some schools questioned the need for the early transition of bright children even when they were fluent as they still benefited from improvements in other subjects, the main issue was the lack of more advanced i.t.a. books to stretch such children
Exploiting i.t.a; resources and the ways of teaching
editProfessor Warburton found the use of i.t.a. was not being exploited to its full potential due to missing resources and not using the best ways of teaching with i.t.a: -
The regularity of the i.t.a. led children to naturally & spontaneously discover the phonetic connection between letters and sounds, known as incidental phonics[60]. Teachers responded by teaching using phonics at an earlier stage than with normal t.o.[61][62]
Vera Southgate described examples of stunned reception class teachers would had put up name tags and posters in i.t.a. in preparation for reading instruction but the children worked out the basics of reading for themselves due to the regularity of i.t.a. which was unheard of in t.o.[63]
The findings on using the i.t.a. with incidental phonics was a boon for progressive schools using the English Language Arts.
Researchers such as Diack (1967) & Stott (1965) concluded that superiority of the i.t.a. over t.o. was simply due the fact it facilitated the use of the phonics method[59].
The researchers Cartwright & Jones (1967) took this thinking to its natural conclusion; the best literacy outcomes would be achieved by the explicit teaching of phonics from the earliest possible age using the i.t.a. and a new basal reading scheme designed from the ground-up for that purpose[59]. At the time, there was no i.t.a. phonic reading scheme equivalent to the Lippincott or Royal Road Readers, both pure phonics schemes in t.o. These schemes could not simply be transliterated into i.t.a. due to the different letters & spelling patterns mapping to phonemes. A brand new phonics basal reading scheme had to be designed from the ground upwards. Out of the 276[64] teachers interviewed, only one applied the i.t.a. using explicit phonics instruction from the outset.
Materials
editBasal readers and other supplementary books were transliterated into i.t.a. but it became apparent that this was constraining the i.t.a.'s potential. Children's reading books had developed over time to use simple words in t.o. and gradually add more and more complex words as the child advanced through reading schemes. This was unnecessary in i.t.a. as most words were simple & regular, instead the books needed to reflect the child's spoken vocabulary and build with that vocabulary. i.t.a. had fundamentally shifted the limitation to reading from the written word to the spoken word. Unfortunately the cost of re-writing the literary works aimed at early age children instead of transliterating these works was prohibitive.
The i.t.a. had retained a considerable level or irregularity in order to ease the transition to t.o. The evidence from its use indicated that transition was effortless but the persistence of irregularity caused the very slowest learners to continue to struggle, although they perfumed better than with t.o. Many teachers wanted a completely regular and phonetically pure orthography. Similarly, many educationalists challenged the design of the i.t.a. and also called for a much simpler phonetic orthography than the i.t.a. Unfortunately, the i.t.a. suffered from the same issue as t.o. in so much as i.t.a. adoption became widespread with a body of literary works and teachers trained to use it, so it became too problematic to change apart from minor embellishments which Pitman made.
Procedures
editThe research had shown that children taught with i.t.a. at the earliest age performed significantly better than equivalent aged children using t.o. hence there was no excuse to delay the start of reading instruction when using i.t.a. Indeed, this was an imperative to ensure the slowest learners had reached fluency & transitioned before their move to Junior school. The second procedural change to unlock the potential of i.t.a. was to introduce free writing early which was simply not possible in t.o.
Monitoring
editSchools needed to be monitored through systematic testing as there was no mechanism for schools to measure their performance against benchmarks or incentive to improve the literacy outcomes of the children in their care.[66]
Slow Learners
editA very small number of children who would have failed to read under t.o. were able to read in i.t.a. but not to the extent where they were transitioned to t.o. Vera Southgate described an example where a girl from a lower socio-economic group had two illiterate parents and two illiterate older siblings who had failed to learn to read in t.o. and so had passed on to an educationally sub-normal school (E.S.N.), uncontested. The girl on the other hand was taught in i.t.a. and so was the first member of the family to be able to read books albeit because the rest of the family was illiterate they were unaware the script she was reading was not standard English,. The question was raised as to whether she should go to E.S.N. despite the fact she could read or a normal school despite the fact she could not read in t.o..
Other findings from using i.t.a.
editThe study of the i.t.a. generated a great deal of research into the teaching of reading & writing to children, this consisted of comparing experimental groups with control groups. Although the i.t.a. was the original subject of study, the research allowed the way literacy was taught in general to be studied in depth and the findings were unexpected: -
Prior Assumptions
editIt was assumed that variations in the performance of children to read and write would be explained by factors such as the socio-economic status of parents, class sizes, school facilities & the general learning environment instead the research found the attitude of the head teachers, methods deployed, materials available and the teaching procedures used were the principle factors which affected the performance of schools, all of these were in the gift of the education authorities to improve. The ministry of educations previous drives to improve the education of young children had centred on nutrition (providing free school milk and food), environment (playgrounds, sports facilities, libraries and swimming pools), school infrastructure (school buildings, classroom facilities) and an adequate supply of teachers.
Teaching Procedures
editThe success of experienced teachers versus inexperienced was demonstrably shown by the research. Sacrificing the education of some unlucky children to inexperienced teachers in order for those teachers to gain experience was considered as unacceptable. The problem was tracked back to the teacher training colleges which were found wanting in the level of training given in language, linguistics and reading instruction methods, often only a few weeks of such education was provided when they should have formed a considerable proportion of the curriculum. Both the i.t.a. training and practical experience in teaching with another orthography was thought by many researchers to uplift the knowledge and skills of those teachers.
In addition, some teachers were simply lacking in the capabilities needed to teach literacy which was clearly an under-estimated skill hence the qualification criteria for entry as trainee teachers needed to be raised.
Researchers had pointed to the fact that the i.t.a. had regularised the orthography of 'look & say' readers so many teachers were now spontaneously able to start to use phonics as their method of choice for teaching literacy. The experiments had also evidenced the fact that 'look & say' provided only a very short-term advantage to phonics but that children using the phonics method soon caught-up and over took. Warburton was of the opinion that measuring early progress was irrelevant and instead the final outcome of early education was important and so signaled that phonics was an improvement over 'look & say'.
Many of the researchers also questioned the compromised design of the i.t.a. which was focused on improving the outcome from using 'look & say', they argued that another design which was phonetically pure would enable the phonics methods to gain considerably more improvements than the i.t.a. achieved with 'look & say'.
On progressive methods, Warburton and the other researchers were scathing, they simply did not work.
Materials
editThe researchers found surprising large disparities between the different reading schemes
Decline of the i.t.a.
editOver the two decades of use, the i.t.a. became inextricably associated with phonics, so when the pendulum of educational theory swung once again back to whole-of-word in the 1980s[68], i.t.a. fell out of fashion and fell into terminal decline in most English speaking countries. It continues in the United States of America under the i.t.a. foundation for the niche charitable purpose of remediating dyslexia. Ironically the cause of i.t.a,'s decline, the whole-of-word method, has now itself been discredited and educational theory has yet again swung back to using phonics[68].
Out of the 119 class & head teachers using i.t.a. polled by Professor Warburton in 1969, only 2 voiced a preference to revert to t.o. One was a junior teacher who had only ever taught in i.t.a. The other suffered the strain of handling both i.t.a. and t.o. children because the teacher's school organisation had not been changed to streaming for the i.t.a. The vast majority of the teachers voiced dismay at the thought of going back to using t.o. instead of i.t.a.
Professor Warburton also asked the few schools who had discontinued use of i.t.a. for the reasons why, and the responses[69] were: -
- The extra cost of books and staffing i.t.a. teachers
- The school organisation was not conducive to using i.t.a.
- The appointment of a new head teacher who was opposed to using an interim orthography
- Not a single school expressed dissatisfaction in the i.t.a.'s performance in teaching literacy
Schools had an incentive to reduce costs & eliminate organisational limitations so the promises of the new whole-of-word methods in the 1980s were appealing.[70]
See also
editNotes
editReferences
edit- ^ a b Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 2 IV Results obtained from questionnaires". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools Council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 9.
- ^ a b Tanyzer, Dr H.J.; Block, Dr J.R. (1969). "i.t.a. Research in the United States; - development of i.t.a. in the United States". i.t.a. Foundation of Hofsra University, New York: 322 – via Appendix III of Alphabets & Reading by Sir James Pitman and John St. John.
- ^ "Initial Teaching Alphabet", Wikipedia, 2021-05-23, retrieved 2021-07-17
- ^ "Initial Teaching Alphabet", Wikipedia, 2021-05-23, retrieved 2021-07-17
- ^ Downing, John; Latham, Wiliiam (1967). Evaluating the Initial Teaching Alphabet. London: Cassell. pp. 75–76.
- ^ a b Ministry of Education (1956). "Standards of Reading". Pamphlet No. 32.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite journal requires|journal=
(help) - ^ Downing, John (1967). Evaluating the Initial Teaching Alphabet. Worcester: Cassell. p. 125.
- ^ "History of Phonics". This History of Phonics. Memoria Press. Retrieved 19 June 2021.
{{cite web}}
: Check date values in:|date=
(help)CS1 maint: url-status (link) - ^ Downing, John (1967). Evaluating the Initial Teaching Alphabet. Worcester: Cassell. p. 132.
- ^ "School Leaving Age". Politics Reference.
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: url-status (link) - ^ "Education - Growth". Education England.
- ^ "Budget Battles - Horsbrugh". Education England.
- ^ "Spelling Reform Bill Volume 462: defeated on 2nd reading on Friday 11 March 1949". Hansard. Retrieved 23 May 2021.
- ^ "Simplified Spelling Bill - Friday 27 February 1953 - Hansard - UK Parliament". hansard.parliament.uk. Retrieved 2021-07-08.
- ^ "Bath (aka Pitman) Printing Press". Heritage Gateway. Retrieved 8 July 2021.
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: url-status (link) - ^ Pitman's speech at the inaugral Mont Follick lecture at Manchester University in 1964
- ^ Downing, John (1967). "How i.t.a. became the 'System of Simplified Spelling' to be used in the experiment". Evaluating the initial teaching alphabet. Cassell. pp. 68–75.
- ^ Downing, Dr John (1967). "The beginning of the i.t.a. research project". Evaluation of the initial teaching alphabet. London: Cassell. pp. 80–91.
- ^ "Obituaries". New York Times. 11 December 1958. Retrieved 17 July 2021.
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: url-status (link) - ^ Downing, Dr John (1967). Evaluation of the initial teaching alphabet. London: Cassell. p. 76.
- ^ Downing, Dr John (1967). Evaluation of the initial teaching alphabet. London: Cassell. p. 80.
- ^ "Reasons for Initiating an Investigation into the Early Stages of Learning to Read". Pamphlet Issued by the University of London Institute of Education in association with The National Foundation for Educational Research in England & Wales. June 1960.
- ^ Downing, John (1967). Evaluating the Initial Teaching Alphabet. Worcester: Cassell. pp. 127–128.
- ^ Downing, John (1967). Evaluating the Initial Teaching Alphabet. Worcester: Cassell. p. 223.
- ^ Downing, John (1967). Evaluating the Initial Teaching Alphabet. Worcester. p. 155.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link) - ^ Downing, John (1967). Evaluating the initial teaching alphabet. Worcester: Cassell. pp. 171–172.
- ^ Downing, Dr John (1967). "How i.t.a. was designed". Evaluating the initial teaching alphabet. Cassell. pp. 75–80.
- ^ Pitman, James (1969). Alphabets & Reading. Bristol, Great Britain: Pitman Press. p. 144. ISBN 0-273-43343-1.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1967). "Chapter 17 IV. Summaries of seventeen researches". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 202–226.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 5 IV - Headteachers' intentions of continuing to use i.t.a. & V. Teachers choice of i.t.a. or t.o.". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 36–37.
- ^ a b Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 8 (D) VI. Advantages and disadvantages of i.t.a.". i.t.a. an independent evaluation. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 86–88.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 6 II A (2) A good beginning". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 43–44.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 18 II. The findings of seventeen researches". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 230–235.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter II 6 C (2) What children read & (3) When children read". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 54–57.
- ^ a b Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 7.1. Free Writing". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 67–70.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 8 1.A.(1) Effect on other subjects; - children's spoken language". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 75–76.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 6.II.3 A better attitude to reading". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 45.
The independence of children who began to read with i.t.a., in contrast to the dependence of many children on the teacher when t.o. was the medium employed, was noted by most teachers
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 8 1 A. Effect on other subjects". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 75.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 8 1. A (2) Effect on other subjects; - Mathematics". i.t.a. an evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 76.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 6 II A (3) A better attitude to reading". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 44–45.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank (1969). "Chapter 7 II. Spelling". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 71–73.
- ^ a b Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 6 (II) b (2) Decrease in non-readers". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the Schools Council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 46–47.
- ^ a b Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 8 IV A. Effect of i.t.a. on dull children". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London. p. 82.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link) - ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 11 IV A. The early stages of reading". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 135–136.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank (1969). "Chapter 8 I.A(1) Children's spoken language". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 75.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 8 III A. Advantages regarding teachers". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 79.
- ^ a b c d Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 6 II C (b) Later changes in material, methods & procedures". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 50.
- ^ a b c Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 10 II 2(b) Disadvantages (vii)". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 118.
- ^ a b c Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 12.II.B.2. Disadvantages from parent's perspective". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 142.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter12.I.C Parent's evidence". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 139.
- ^ a b Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 12.I. B & C - Parent's evidence". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 138–139.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 12. II. A. Discussions with parents". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. from parent's letter supplied by James Pitman. London: Murray Chambers. p. 140.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 8 III.C Frequent staff changes". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 80–82.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 13 (II) Schools using & discarding the i.t.a.". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 145.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1967). "Chapter 10.V.C More Detailed Observations (f)". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 125.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 11.IV.B. Reading after the first two years". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 136.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 8. III. B Disadvantages for teachers". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Harper. p. 80.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1967). "Chapter 6 III.D Teacher's Difficulties". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 64.
- ^ a b c Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 19 I.H. Teaching Methods". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 258–261.
the superiority of i.t.a. does not arise from the alphabet used but from the fact that it facilitates a phonic method of teaching.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 6.II.C (1) Material, Methods & Procedures". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 53.
What is done, as a result of its regularity, is to spark off an enormous increase in incidental phonic work, springing mainly from the children's own observations and interest. The teacher has utilised these moments to reinforce their learning by incidental teaching.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 52.
According to the majority of teachers, the recognition of letters and sounds led fairly rapidly to the blending of sounds to form words and to an appreciation of the phonic rules...phonic work automatically starts earlier
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 259.
In Downing's (1967) research. five out of twelve teachers said that with the i.t.a. class they used phonic methods earlier than with the t.o. class in response to the children's interest.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 6 II C (1) Material, Methods & Procedures". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 51–52.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Appendix C - Table D/4 - Teachers Interviewed". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 300.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 11.II.A.2. Possible alternatives to i.t.a. (a) Criticisms of i.t.a.". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. pp. 129–130.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 6 II B (1) Records of reading progress". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 46.
- ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1967). "Chapter 6 IV Summing up on reading". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 65.
- ^ a b "Reading Wars". PBS. Retrieved 21 July 2021.
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: url-status (link) - ^ Warburton, Professor Frank; Southgate, Vera (1969). "Chapter 2 IV Results obtained from questionnaires". i.t.a. an independent evaluation for the schools council. London: Murray Chambers. p. 11.
- ^ Warburton & Southgate 1969, p. 78. sfn error: multiple targets (35×): CITEREFWarburtonSouthgate1969 (help)
External links
edit- "Pitman Initial Teaching Alphabet (i.t.a.)". Omniglot. Retrieved 31 December 2014.
- "Initial Teaching Alphabet Foundation". ITA Foundation. Retrieved 23 May 2021.
- "Discussion group of people taught by i.t.a." Old Ladybird Books. Retrieved 17 July 2021.
- "i.t.a. book covers". Pinterest. Retrieved 17 July 2021.
GOVERMENT REPORTS
- "The Plowden Report (1967) - Children and their Primary Schools". Plowden Report. Retrieved 23 May 2021.
PARLIAMENTRY DEBATES - INITIAL TEACHING ALPHABET
- "Initial Teaching Alphabet - Volume 793: debated on Wednesday 17 December 1969". Hansard. Retrieved 25 May 2021.
- "Initial Teaching Alphabet - Volume 791: debated on Wednesday 19 November 1969". Hansard. Retrieved 25 May 2021.
- "Initial Teaching Alphabet - Volume 791: debated on Monday 17 November 1969". Hansard. Retrieved 25 May 2021.
- "Initial Teaching Alphabet - Volume 750: debated on Thursday 20 July 1967". Hansard. Retrieved 25 May 2021.
- "Initial Teaching Alphabet - Volume 745: debated on Friday 21 April 1967". Hansard. Retrieved 25 May 2021.
- "Initial Teaching Alphabet - Volume 710: debated on Wednesday 7 April 1965". Hansard. Retrieved 25 May 2021.
- "Initial Teaching Alphabet - Volume 708: debated on Thursday 18 March 1965". Hansard. Retrieved 25 May 2021.
- "Initial Teaching Alphabet - Volume 692: debated on Thursday 26 March 1964". Hansard. Retrieved 25 May 2021.
- "Education (Initial Teaching Alphabet) - Volume 691: debated on Thursday 19 March 1964". Hansard. Retrieved 25 May 2021.
- "Initial Teaching Alphabet System - Volume 680: debated on Thursday 11 July 1963". Hansard. Retrieved 25 May 2021.
PRIVATE MEMBERS BILL - SIMPLIFIED SPELLING
- "Simplified Spelling Bill Volume 511: passed for 2nd reading on Friday 27 February 1953". Hansard. Retrieved 23 May 2021.
- "Simplified Spelling Bill - Volume 507: debated on Wednesday 19 November 1952". Hansard. Retrieved 23 May 2021.
- "Simplified English Spelling - Volume 496: debated on Thursday 21 February 1952". Hansard. Retrieved 23 May 2021.
- "Simplified English Spelling - Volume 497: debated on Thursday 6 March 1952". Hansard. Retrieved 25 May 2021.
PRIVATE MEMBERS BILL - SPELLING REFORM
- "Spelling Reform Bill Volume 462: defeated on 2nd reading on Friday 11 March 1949". Hansard. Retrieved 23 May 2021.
- "Spelling Reform Bill - Volume 460: debated on Friday 28 January 1949". Hansard. Retrieved 23 May 2021.
Category:Phonics
Category:Phonetic alphabets
Category:English spelling reform
Category:Learning
Category:Reading (process)